Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:1 (2017) ► pp.107128
References
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernandez, A., & Pizzamiglio, L.
(1996) Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 581, 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M., Burani, C., Laudanna, A., Marconi, L., Ratti, D., Rolando, C., & Thornton, A. M.
(2005) Corpus e Lessico di Frequenza dell’Italiano Scritto (CoLFIS). [URL]
Bordag, D., & Pechmann, T.
(2007) Factors influencing L2 gender processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 299–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bordag, D., Opitz, A., & Pechmann, T.
(2006) Gender processing in first and second languages: The role of noun termination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 321, 1090–1101.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A.
(1997) How many levels of processing are there in lexical access?. Cognitive neuropsychology, 141, 177–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caramazza, A., & Miozzo, M.
(1997) The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the tip-of-the-tongue’ phenomenon. Cognition, 641, 309–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, A.
(1998) How lexical semantics constrains inflectional allomorphy. In Yearbook of Morphology 1997, 1–24. Springer Netherlands. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C. Leonard, L., Volterra, V., & Campagnoli, M. G.
(1993) Toward mastery of Italian morphology: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Child Language, 201, 377–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chini, M.
(1995) Genere grammaticale e acquisizione. Aspetti della morfologia nominale in italiano L2. Milano: Franco Angeli.Google Scholar
Colé, P., Pynte, J. & Andriamamonjy, P.
(2003) Effect of grammatical gender on visual word recognition: Evidence from lexical decision and eye movement experiments. Perception & Psychophysics, 651, 407–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R.
(2005) Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture – word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 531, 42–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dardano, M., & Trifone, M.
(1985) La lingua italiana. Bologna: Zanichelli.Google Scholar
De Martino, M., & Laudanna, A.
(2012) Il genere dei nomi italiani: dati sperimentali e distribuzionali, in S. Ferreri (Ed.), Lessico e Lessicologia (pp. 343–354). Roma: Bulzoni.Google Scholar
De Martino, M., Bracco, G., & Laudanna, A.
(2011) The activation of grammatical gender information in processing Italian nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 261, 745–776. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Di Domenico, E.
(1997) Per una teoria del genere grammaticale. Padova: Unipress.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F.
(2001) Morphological or syntactic deficits in near-native speakers? An assessment of some current proposals. Second Language Research, 171, 213–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., & Frost, R.
(2001) Two routes to grammatical gender: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 301, 627–651. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F., Dommergues, .J. Y., Cornu, E., Guillelmon, D., & Besson, C.
(1994) The gender-marking effect in spoken word recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 561, 590–598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guillelmon, D., & Grosjean, F.
(2001) The gender marking effect in spoken word recognition: The case of bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 291, 503–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halle, M., & Marantz, A.
(1994) Some key features of Distributed Morphology. MIT working papers in linguistics, 211, 275–288.Google Scholar
Harley, H., & Ritter, E.
(2002) Person and number in pronouns: A feature-geometric analysis. Language, 781, 482–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. W.
(1991) The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 221, 27–62.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., Kotz, S. A., Hofmann, J., Valentin, V. V., Dapretto, M., & Bookheimer, S. Y.
(2004) The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. Neuroreport, 151, 863–866. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, C.
(2010) On the nature of phonological cues in the acquisition of French gender categories: Evidence from instance-based learning models. Lingua, 1201, 879–900. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, C.
(2008) Morphological variability in the comprehension of agreement: An argument for representation over computation. Second Language Research, 241, 459–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Padovani, R., & Cacciari, C.
(2003) Il ruolo della trasparenza morfologica nel riconoscimento di parole in italiano. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 41, 749–771.Google Scholar
Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., Leoncini, D., Cubelli, R., & Job, R.
(2011) Differential effects of grammatical gender and gender inflection in bare noun production. British Journal of Psychology, 1021, 19–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M.
(1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 181, 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1998) Words and rules. Lingua, 1061, 219–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Postiglione, F., De Martino, M., Bracco, G., & Laudanna, A.
(2014) Frequency distribution of inflectional classes and grammatical gender: a lexical database for Italian nouns. Poster presented at Secondo Convegno Interannuale della Società di Linguistica Italiana (SLI): Teorie e approcci usage-based in linguistica.Google Scholar
Serianni, L.
(1988) Grammatica italiana. Italiano comune e lingua letteraria. Suoni, forme, costrutti. Torino: Utet.Google Scholar
Spinelli, E., Meunier, F. & Seigneuric, A.
(2006) Spoken word recognition with gender-marked context. The Mental Lexicon, 11, 277–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Taft, M., & Meunier, F.
(1998) Lexical representation of gender: A quasi-regular domain. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 271, 23–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. & Hulk, A.
(2013) Grammatical gender and the notion of default: Insights from language acquisition. Lingua, 1371, 128–144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowsk-Macgregor, M., & Leung, Y. K. I.
(2004) Gender and number agreement in non native Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 251, 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

De Martino, Maria, Azzurra Mancuso & Alessandro Laudanna
2017. Variabili Rilevanti nella Rappresentazione delle Parole nel Lessico Mentale: Dati Psicolinguistici da una Banca-Dati di Nomi e Verbi Italiani. In Proceedings of the Fourth Italian Conference on Computational Linguistics CLiC-it 2017,  pp. 125 ff. DOI logo
Paolieri, Daniela, Josep Demestre, Marc Guasch, Teresa Bajo & Pilar Ferré
2020. The gender congruency effect in Catalan–Spanish bilinguals: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23:5  pp. 1045 ff. DOI logo
Pescuma, Valentina Nicole, Chiara Zanini, Davide Crepaldi & Francesca Franzon
2021. Form and Function: A Study on the Distribution of the Inflectional Endings in Italian Nouns and Adjectives. Frontiers in Psychology 12 DOI logo
Russo, Andrea G., Fabrizio Esposito, Alessandro Laudanna, Azzurra Mancuso, Francesco Di Salle, Annibale Elia & Maria De Martino
2021. The neural substrate of noun morphological inflection: A rapid event-related fMRI study in Italian. Neuropsychologia 151  pp. 107699 ff. DOI logo
Sá-Leite, A. R. & S. Lago
2024. The role of word form in gender processing during lexical access: A theoretical review and novel proposal in language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review DOI logo
Sá-Leite, Ana Rita, Karlos Luna, Isabel Fraga & Montserrat Comesaña
2020. The gender congruency effect across languages in bilinguals: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 27:4  pp. 677 ff. DOI logo
Velnić, Marta
2020. Acquisition of a Transparent Gender System: A Comparison of Italian and Croatian. Frontiers in Psychology 11 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.