Article published in:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:1 (2017) ► pp. 129158

Full-text

Poor performance on the retention of phonemes’ serial order in short-term memory reflects young children’s poor reading skills
References

References

Aarnoutse, C., van Leeuwe, J., & Verhoeven, L.
(2005) Early Literacy from a Longitudinal Perspective. Educational Research and Evaluation, 11, 253–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Attout, L., & Majerus, S.
(2015) Working memory deficits in developmental dyscalculia: the importance of serial order. Child Neuropsychology, 21, 432–450. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baird, G., Slonims, V., Simonoff, E., & Dworzynski, K.
(2011) Impairment in non-word repetition: a marker for language impairment or reading impairment? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 53, 711–716. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D. M., Maechler, M., & Dai, B.
(2008) lme4: Linear mixed effects models using S4 classes (R Package Version 0.999375-25). [Software]. Retrieved from http://​cran​.r​-project​.org​/web​/packages​/lme4​/index​.html.
[ p. 155 ]
Beneventi, H., Tonnessen, F. E., & Ersland, L.
(2009) Dyslexic children show short-term memory deficits in phonological storage and serial rehearsal: an fMRI study. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119, 2017–2043. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Binamé, F., & Poncelet, M.
(2016) Order short-term memory capacity predicts nonword reading and spelling in first and second grade. Reading and Writing, 29, 1–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boada, R., & Pennington, B. F.
(2006) Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 153–193. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boets, B.
(2006) Early literacy development in children at risk for dyslexia. A longitudinal study of the general magnocellular theory (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Leuven, Belgium.Google Scholar
de Bree, E. H., Rispens, J., & Gerrits, E.
(2007) Non-word repetition in Dutch children with (a risk of) dyslexia and SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 935–944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brus, B.Th., & Voeten, M. J. M.
(1997) Een Minuut Test. Harcourt: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J.
(2004) Language and phonological skills in children at high-risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 631–640. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M.
(2005) Individual differences in RAN and reading: a response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cumming, N., Page, M., & Norris, D.
(2003) Testing a positional model of the Hebb effect. Memory, 11, 43–63. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Jong, P. F.
(2011) What Discrete and Serial Rapid Automatized Naming Can Reveal About Reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 314–337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., Yaghoub-Zadeh, & Shanahan, T.
(2001) Phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C.
(1996) Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 453–485. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998) When reading is “readn” or somthn. Distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 149–153. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C., Borstrøm, I., & Petersen, D. K.
(1998) Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten. The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 36–60. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C., & Jensen, M. N.
(2005) Quality of phonological representations, verbal learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375–384. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P.
(2003) Examining the relationship between word learning, nonword repetition, and immediate serial recall in adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1213–1236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, P., Lipinski, J., Abbs, B., & Lin, P-H.
(2005) Serial Position Effects in Nonword Repetition. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 141–162. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 156 ]
Hachmann, W., Bogaerts, L., Szmalec A., Woumans, E., Duyck, W., & Job, R.
(2014) Short-term memory for order but not for item information is impaired in developmental dyslexia. Annals of dyslexia, 64, 121–136. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., & Snowling, M.
(1992) Phonological deficits in dyslexia: a reappraisal of the verbal deficit hypothesis. In N. Singh & I. Beale (Eds.), 590 Current perspectives in learning disabilities (pp. 270–331). New York: Springer-Verlag. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jaroslawska, A. J., Gathercole, S. E., Logie, M., & Holmes, J.
(2016) Following instructions in a virtual school: Does working memory play a role? Memory & Cognition, 44, 580–589. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P., Friederici, A., Wessels, J., Svenkerud, V., & Jusczyk, A. M.
(1993) Infants’ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 402–420. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J.
(1994) Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630–645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laasonen, M., Virsu, V., Oinonen, S., Sandbacka, M., Salakari, A., & Service, E.
(2012) Phonological and sensory short-term memory are correlates and both affected in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 25, 2247–2273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A.
(2003) A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., & Cowan, N.
(2016) The nature of verbal short-term impairment in dyslexia: The importance of serial order. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–8. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., Poncelet, M., Van der Linden, M., & Weekes, B.
(2008) Lexical learning in bilingual adults: the relative importance of short-term memory for serial order and phonological knowledge. Cognition, 107, 395–419. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., Belayachi, S., De Smedt, B., Leclercq, A. L., Martinez, T., Schmidt, C., et al.
(2008) Neural networks for short-term memory for order differentiate high and low proficiency bilinguals. Neuroimage, 42, 1698–1713. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Majerus S., D'Argembeau A., Martinez T., Belayachi S., Van der Linden M., Collette F., et al.
(2010) The commonality of neural networks for verbal and visual short-term memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2570–2593. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., & Poncelet, M.
(2012a) The contribution of short-term memory for serial order to early reading acquisition: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111, 708–723. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martinez Perez, T., Majerus, S., Mahot, A., & Poncelet, M.
(2012b) Evidence for a specific impairment of serial order short-term memory in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 18, 94–109. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McKelvie, S. J.
(1987) Learning and awareness in the Hebb digits task. Journal of General Psychology, 114, 75–88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A.
(2011) Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 1, 114–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Molenberghs, G., & Verbeke, G.
(2005) Models for Discrete Longitudinal Data. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Munson, B., Swenson, C. L., & Manthei, S. C.
(2005) Lexical and phonological organization in children: evidence from repetition tasks. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 108–123. Crossref(2005/009)Google Scholar
Nation, K., & Hulme, C.
(2011) Learning to read changes children’s phonological skills: Evidence from a latent variable longitudinal study of reading and nonword repetition. Developmental Science, 14, 649–659. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 157 ]
Nairne, J. S., & Kelley, M. R.
(2004) Separating item and order information through process dissociation. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 113–133. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Page, M., & Norris, D.
(2009) Is there a common mechanism underlying word-form learning and the Hebb repetition effect? Experimental data and a modelling framework. In A. Thorn & M. Page (Eds.). Interactions between short-term and long-term memory in the verbal domain (pp. 136–156), Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Pratt, A., & Brady, S.
(1988) Relation of Phonological Awareness to Reading Disability in Children and Adults. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 319–323. Crossref.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U.
(2003) Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G.
(2008) What phonological deficit? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 129–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rispens, J., & Parigger, E.
(2010) Non-word repetition in Dutch-speaking children with specific language impairment with and without reading problems. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 177–188. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, E. K., & Joanisse, M. F.
(2010) Spoken sentence comprehension in children with dyslexia and language impairment: The roles of syntax and working memory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 141–165. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Romani, C., Tsouknida, E., & Olson, A.
(2015) Encoding order and developmental dyslexia: a family of skills predicting different orthographic components. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 99–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schraeyen, K., Geudens, A., Ghesquière, P., & Sandra, D.
under review). Serial order effects in young adults with dyslexia: new perspectives from nonword repetition errors. Manuscript under review.
Smith-Spark, J., & Fisk, J.
(2007) Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15, 34–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U.
(2003) Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Staels, E., & Van den Broeck, W.
(2014) Order short-term memory is not impaired in dyslexia and does not affect orthographic learning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) No solid empirical evidence for the SOLID (Serial order learning impairment) hypothesis of dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 650–669. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S.
(1994) Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Storkel, H. L.
(2001) Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1321–1337. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bos, K. P., Spelberg, H. C., Scheepstra, A. J. M., & de Vries, J. C.
(1998) De klepel pseudowoordentest. Harcourt: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Van den Broeck, W., Geudens, A., & Van den Bos, K. P.
(2010) The nonword-reading deficit of disabled readers: A developmental interpretation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 717–734. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G.
(2000) Linear Mixed Models for Longitudinal Data. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
[ p. 158 ]
Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K.
(1987) The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Kolinsky, Régine, Rosângela Gabriel, Catherine Demoulin, Marilane Maria Gregory, Kadine Saraiva de Carvalho & José Morais
2020. The influence of age, schooling, literacy, and socioeconomic status on serial-order memory. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science 4:3  pp. 343 ff. Crossref logo
Ordonez Magro, Laura, Steve Majerus, Lucie Attout, Martine Poncelet, Eleonore H.M. Smalle & Arnaud Szmalec
2021. Do serial order short-term memory and long-term learning abilities predict spelling skills in school-age children?. Cognition 206  pp. 104479 ff. Crossref logo
SCHRAEYEN, KIRSTEN, WIM VAN DER ELST, ASTRID GEUDENS, POL GHESQUIÈRE & DOMINIEK SANDRA
2018. Beyond global differences between monolingual and bilingual children on the nonword repetition task: retention skills for phonemes’ identity and serial order. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21:2  pp. 403 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 30 august 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.