Article published In:
The Mental Lexicon
Vol. 12:3 (2017) ► pp.404430
References
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H.
(2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clopper, C. G., & Walker, A.
(2017) Effects of lexical competition and dialect exposure on phonological priming. Language and Speech, 60(1), 85–109. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., Sebastiân-Gallés, N., Soler-Vilageliu, O., & van Ooijen, B.
(2000) Constraints of vowels and consonants on lexical selection: Cross-linguistic comparisons. Memory & Cognition, 28(5), 746–755. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Delle Luche, C., Poltrock, S., Goslin, J., New, B., Floccia, C., & Nazzi, T.
(2014) Differential processing of consonants and vowels in the auditory modality: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Memory and Language, 721, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F.
(2008) Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(10), 1893–1906. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S.
(2008) Phonological priming in auditory word recognition: When both controlled and automatic processes are responsible for the effects. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(1), 33–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S., Dumon, A., & Nguyen, N.
(2015) Does a change in talker identity help listeners resolve lexical competition? evidence from phonological priming. In Proceedings of the eighteenth international congress of the phonetic sciences.Google Scholar
Dufour, S., & Frauenfelder, U. H.
(2016) Inhibitory phonetic priming: Where does the effect come from? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(1), 180–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S., & Nguyen, N.
(2017) Does talker-specific information influence lexical competition? evidence from phonological priming. Cognitive Science, 41(8), 2221–2233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S., & Peereman, R.
(2009) Competition effects in phonological priming: The role of mismatch position between primes and targets. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 381, 475–490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dumay, N., Benraïss, A., Barriol, B., Colin, C., Radeau, M., & Besson, M.
(2001) Behavioral and electrophysiological study of phonological priming between bisyllabic spoken words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(1), 121–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gaskell, M. G., & Dumay, N.
(2003) Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words. Cognition, 891, 105–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. D.
(1998) Signal detection comparisons of phonemic and phonetic priming: The flexible-bias problem. Perception & Psychophysics, 60(6), 952–965. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. D., Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Marcario, J. K.
(1992) Form-based priming in spoken word recognition: The roles of competition and bias. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.Google Scholar
Gray, S., Reiser, M., & Brinkley, S.
(2012) Effect of onset and rhyme pairs in preschoolers with typical development and specific languages impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 551, 32–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hamburger, M., & Slowiaczek, L. M.
(1996) Phonological priming reflects lexical competition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 520–525. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Havy, M., & Nazzi, T.
(2009) Better processing of consonantal over vocalic information in word learning at 16 months of age. Infancy, 14(4), 439–456. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
James, L. E., & Burke, D. M.
(2000) Phonological priming effects on word retrieval and tip-of-the-tongue experiences in young and older adults. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1378–1391.Google Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M.
(2010) Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 627–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowenstamm, J., & Kaye, J.
(1986) Compensatory lengthening in Tiberian Hebrew. In W. L. Wetzels & E. Sezer (Eds.), Studies in compensatory lengthening (pp. 97–132). Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luce, P. A., Goldinger, S. D., Auer, E. T., & Vitevitch, M. S.
(2000) Phonetic priming, neighborhood activation, and PARSYN. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(3), 615–625. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W.
(1990) Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access. In G. T. M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing (pp. 148–172). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
McQueen, J. M., & Sereno, J.
(2005) Cleaving automatic processes from strategic biases in phonological priming. Memory & Cognition, 33(7), 1185–1209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Floccia, C., Moquet, B., & Butler, J.
(2009) Bias for consonantal information over vocalic information in 30-month-olds: Cross-linguistic evidence from french and english. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1021, 522–537. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nespor, M., Pena, M., & Mehler, J.
(2003) On the different roles of vowels and consonants in speech processing and language acquisition. Lingue e linguaggio, 2(2), 203–230.Google Scholar
New, B., Araûjo, V., & Nazzi, T.
(2008) Differential processing of consonants and vowels in lexical access through reading. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1223–1227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., & Matos, R.
(2001) Une base de données lexical du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. L’Année Psychologique, 1011, 447–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., & Cutler, A.
(2002) Bias effects in facilitatory phonological priming. Memory & Cognition, 30(3), 399–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peirce, J. W.
(2009) Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 21, 10.Google Scholar
Prunet, J. -F.
(1987) Liaison and nasalization in French. In C. J. Neidle & R. Nûnez Cedeno (Eds.), Studies in Romance languages (pp. 225–235). Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radeau, M., Besson, M., Fonteneau, E., & Castro, S. L.
(1998) Semantic, repetition and rime priming between spoken words: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Biological Psychology, 481, 183–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radeau, M., Morais, J., & Segui, J.
(1995) Phonological priming between monosyllabic spoken words. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(6), 1297–1311.Google Scholar
Selkirk, E. O.
(1986) Phonology and syntax: the relationship between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, L. M., & Hamburger, M.
(1992) Prelexical facilitation and lexical interference in auditory word recognition. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(6), 1239–1250.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, L. M., McQueen, J. M., Soltano, E. G., & Lynch, M.
(2000) Phonological representations in prelexical speech processing: Evidence from form-based priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 431, 530–560. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slowiaczek, L. M., Nusbaum, H. C., & Pisoni, D. B.
(1987) Phonological priming in auditory word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(1), 64–75.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, L. M., & Pisoni, D. B.
(1986) Effects of phonological similarity on priming in auditory lexical decision. Memory & Cognition, 141, 230–237. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Turnbull, R.
(2017) The role of predictability in intonational variation. Language and Speech, 60(1), 123–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiener, S., & Turnbull, R.
(2016) Constraints of tones, vowels and consonants on lexical selection in Mandarin Chinese. Language and Speech, 59(1), 59–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 3 other publications

Berrebi, Si, Outi Bat-El & Aya Meltzer-Asscher
2023. The roots of consonant bias in semitic languages: a critical review of psycholinguistic studies of languages with non-concatenative morphology. Morphology 33:3  pp. 225 ff. DOI logo
Nieder, Jessica, Ruben van de Vijver & Holger Mitterer
2021. Knowledge of Maltese singular–plural mappings. Morphology 31:2  pp. 147 ff. DOI logo
Sun, Yue & David Poeppel
2023. Syllables and their beginnings have a special role in the mental lexicon. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 120:36 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 6 june 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.