Article published In:
Semantic Considerations of Lexical Processing
[The Mental Lexicon 9:1] 2014
► pp. 4866
References (30)
Baayen, R. H. (2007). LanguageR: Data sets and functions with analyzing linguistic data. R package version 0.1.Google Scholar
2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R . Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language , 591, 390–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM) . Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Bates, D., & Maechler, M. (lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. 2009). R package version 0.999375-32.
Butterworth, B. (1983). Lexical representation. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production , Volume 21, (pp. 257–294). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes , 101, 425–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cover, T. M., & Thomas, J. A. (1991). Elements of information theory . Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fiorentino, R., & Poeppel, D. (2007). Compound words and structure in the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes , 221, 953– 1000. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Shoben, E. J. (1997). Influence of thematic relations on the comprehension of modifier-noun combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 231, 71–87. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2006). Using conceptual combination research to better understand novel compound words. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics , 31, 9–16.Google Scholar
2009). Constituent integration during the processing of compound words: Does it involve the use of relational structures? Journal of Memory and Language , 60(1), 20–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010). Relational competition during compound interpretation. In S. Scalise & I. Vogel (Eds.), Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding (pp. 287–300). 
Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2013). Relation diversity and ease of processing for opaque and transparent English compounds. Submitted for publication.
in press). Conceptual composition: The role of relational competition in the comprehension of modifier-noun phrases and noun-noun compounds. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation , Volume 59 (pp. 97–130). New York: Elsevier.
Gagné, C. L., Spalding, T. L., Figueredo, L., & Mullaly, A. C. (2009). Does snow man prime plastic snow? The effect of constituent position in using relational information during the interpretation of modifier-noun phrases. The Mental Lexicon , 41, 41–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L. (2011). Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language , 651, 406–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Wuggy: A multilingual pseudoword generator. Behavior Research Methods , 421, 627–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2010). Processing trade-offs in the reading of Dutch derived words. Journal of Memory and Language , 62(2), 83–97. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuperman, V., Schreuder, R., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Reading polymorphemic Dutch compounds: Toward a multiple route model of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 351, 876–895. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levi, J. N. (1978). The syntax and semantics of complex nominals . New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Libben, G. (2006). Why study compound processing: An overview of the issues. In G. Libben & G. Jarema (Eds.), The representation and processing of compound words (pp. 1–21). New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
2010). Compound words, semantic transparency, and morphological transcendence. In S. Olson (Ed.), New impulses in word-formation (Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft 17) (pp. 283–315). Hamburg: Buske.Google Scholar
Pham, H., & Baayen, R. H. (2013). Semantic relations and compound transparency: A regression study in CARIN theory. Psychologia , 46(4), 455–478.
Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effects models in S and S-Plus . New York, NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sandra, D. (1990). On the representation and processing of compound words: Automatic access to constituent morphemes does not occur. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 42A1, 529–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spalding, T. L., & Gagné, C. L. (2008). CARIN theory reanalysis reanalyzed: A comment on Maguire, Devereux, Costello, and Cater 2007. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 341, 1573– 1578. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2011). Relation priming in established compounds: Facilitation? Memory & Cognition , 391, 1472–1486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spalding, T. L., Gagné, C. L., Mullaly, A. C., & Ji, H. (2010). Relation-based interpretation of noun-noun phrases: A new theoretical approach. In S. Olson (Ed.), New impulses in word-formation (Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft 17) (pp. 283–315). Hamburg: Buske.Google Scholar
Zwitserlood, P. (1994). The role of semantic transparency in the processing and representation of Dutch compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes , 9(3), 341–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (10)

Cited by ten other publications

Benjamin, Shaina & Daniel Schmidtke
2023. Conceptual combination during novel and existing compound word reading in context: A self-paced reading study. Memory & Cognition 51:5  pp. 1170 ff. DOI logo
Cruz, Karen Pérez, Chelsa Patel, Jazlynn Steinbach, Mohamed Barre, Holly Kibbins, Dixie Wong, Alexander Taikh, Christina L. Gagné & Thomas L. Spalding
2022. Is meaning construction attempted during the processing of pseudo-compounds?. The Mental Lexicon 17:2  pp. 277 ff. DOI logo
Günther, Fritz & Marco Marelli
2022. Patterns in CAOSS: Distributed representations predict variation in relational interpretations for familiar and novel compound words. Cognitive Psychology 134  pp. 101471 ff. DOI logo
Creemers, Ava & David Embick
2021. Retrieving stem meanings in opaque words during auditory lexical processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 36:9  pp. 1107 ff. DOI logo
Hennecke, Inga & Harald Baayen
2021. Romance N Prep N constructions in visual word recognition. The Mental Lexicon 16:1  pp. 98 ff. DOI logo
Schmidtke, Daniel, Christina L. Gagné, Victor Kuperman, Thomas L. Spalding & Benjamin V. Tucker
2018. Conceptual relations compete during auditory and visual compound word recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 33:7  pp. 923 ff. DOI logo
Schmidtke, Daniel, Christina L. Gagné, Victor Kuperman & Thomas L. Spalding
2018. Language experience shapes relational knowledge of compound words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 25:4  pp. 1468 ff. DOI logo
Marelli, Marco, Christina L. Gagné & Thomas L. Spalding
2017. Compounding as Abstract Operation in Semantic Space: Investigating relational effects through a large-scale, data-driven computational model. Cognition 166  pp. 207 ff. DOI logo
Gagné, Christina L. & Thomas L. Spalding
2016. Written production of English compounds: effects of morphology and semantic transparency. Morphology 26:2  pp. 133 ff. DOI logo
Schmidtke, Daniel, Victor Kuperman, Christina L. Gagné & Thomas L. Spalding
2016. Competition between conceptual relations affects compound recognition: the role of entropy. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23:2  pp. 556 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 december 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.