Part of
Language and Food: Verbal and nonverbal experiences
Edited by Polly E. Szatrowski
[Pragmatics & Beyond New Series 238] 2014
► pp. 211232
References
Barthomeuf, L., Droit-Volet, S., & Rousset, S.
(2011). How emotions expressed by adults' faces affect the desire to eat liked and disliked foods in children compared to adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology . 30(2): 253–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barthomeuf, L., Rousset, S., & Droit-Volet, S.
(2009). Emotion and food: Do the emotions expressed on other people's faces affect the desire to eat liked and disliked food products? Appetite , 52, 27–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L.
(1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of others' emotions. Developmental Psychology , 27(3), 448–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edwards, D.
(1999). Emotion discourse. Culture & Psychology , 5(3), 271–291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edwards, D., & Potter, J.
(2005). Discursive psychology, mental states and descriptions. InH. te Molder & J. Potter (Eds.), Conversation and cognition (pp. 241–259). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V.
(1978). The facial action coding system. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Gagnon, M., Gosselin, P., Hudon-ven der Buhs, I., Larocque, K., & Milliard, K.
(2010). Children's recognition and discrimination of fear and disgust facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour , 34, 27–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M., Cekaite, A., & Goodwin, C.
(2012). Emotion as Stance . InM.-L. Sorjonen & A. Perakyla (Eds.), Emotion in Interaction (pp. 16–41). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goffman, E.
(1981). Forms of talk . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P.
(1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences , 16(5), 701–713. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hepburn, A., & Wiggins, S.
(2005). Size matters: Constructing accountable bodies in NSPCC helpline interaction. Discourse & Society , 16(5), 625–646. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, L.
(2010). “Your head hurts but you're holding your tummy“: The co-production of children's bodily experience through talk and gesture in family interaction . Paper presented at the International Conference on Conversation Analysis, Mannheim, July 2010.Google Scholar
Lerner, G.H., Zimmerman, D.H., & M. Kidwell
(2011) Formal structures of practical tasks: A resource for action in the social life of very young children. InJ. Streeck, C. Goodwin & C. LeBaron (Eds.), Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world . (pp. 44–58). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analysing talk (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Martins, Y., & Pliner, P.
(2006). “Ugh! Thats disgusting!" Identification of the characteristics of foods underlying rejections based on disgust. Appetite , 46, 75–85. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNally, R. J.
(2002). Disgust has arrived. Anxiety Disorders , 16, 561–566. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. I.
(1997). The anatomy of disgust . Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mondloch, C. J.
(2012). Sad or fearful? The influence of body posture on adults' and children's perception of facial displays of emotion Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 111, 180–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P.
(1990). Social and moral aspects of food and eating. InI. Rock (Ed.), The legacy of Solomon Asch: Essays in cognition and social psychology (pp. 97–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E.
(1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review , 94(1), 23–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Fallon, A. E., & Augustoni-Ziskind, M. L.
(1985). The child's conception of food: The development of contamination sensitivity to “disgusting" substances. Developmental Psychology , 21(6), 1075–1079. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C.
(2000). Disgust. InM. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions . (pp. 637–653). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Siegal, M., Fadda, R., & Overton, P. G.
(2011). Contamination sensitivity and the development of disease-avoidant behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B , 366, 3427–3432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simpson, J., Anthony, S. H., Schmeer, S., & Overton, P. G.
(2007). Food-related contextual factors substantially modify the disgust response. Food Quality and Preference , 18, 183–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Oaten, M. J., Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M., & Wagland, P.
(2010). Children's response to adult disgust elicitors: Development and acquisition. Developmental Psychology , 46(1), 165–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tenenbaum, H. R., Ford, S., & Alkhedairy, B.
(2011). Telling stories: Gender differences in peers' emotion talk and communication style. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 29, 707–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Toyama, N.
(1999). Developmental changes in the basis of associated contamination thinking. Cognitive Development , 14, 343–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000). Young children's awareness of socially mediated rejection of food: Why is food dropped at the table “dirty”? Cognitive Development , 15, 523–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Viellard, S., & Guidetti, M.
(2009). Children's perception and understanding of (dis)similarities among dynamic bodily/ facial expressions of happiness, pleasure, anger and irritation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 102, 78–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A.
(2004). The relative power of an emotion's facial expression, label, and behavioral consequence to evoke preschoolers' knowledge of its cause. Cognitive Development , 19, 111–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008a). Children's and adults' understanding of the “disgust face”. Cognition and Emotion , 22(8), 1513–1541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008b). Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cognitive Development , 23, 291–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010). The 'disgust face' conveys anger to children. Emotion , 10(4), 455–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). In building a script for an emotion, do preschoolers add its cause before its behavior consequence? Social Development , 20(3), 471–485. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiggins, S.
(2002). Talking with your mouth full: gustatory mmms and the embodiment of pleasure. Research on Language and Social Interaction , 35(3), 311–336. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). The social life of “eugh”: Disgust as assessment in family mealtimes. British Journal of Social Psychology . doi: 10.1111/j.2044–8309.2012.02106.x
Wiggins, S., & Potter, J.
(2008). Discursive psychology. InC. Willig & W. Stainton Rogers (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research in psychology (pp. 73–90). London: Sage. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Choe, Hanwool
2019. Eating together multimodally: Collaborative eating inmukbang, a Korean livestream of eating. Language in Society 48:2  pp. 171 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.