This chapter explores the potential existence of
interactional reciprocity in human–dog interactions by analyzing
experimental data on situations where dog owners fail to produce
reciprocally modified actions for two minutes. We found that their
dogs soon realized the strangeness of the situation. While they
pursued missing feedback with touch, gaze and vocalizations, they
also addressed other humans and requested their attention.
Therefore, the dogs oriented to the lack of reciprocity and
attempted to repair it. We demonstrate that interactional
reciprocity in human–dog interactions can be breached, thus proving
its existence. Moreover, we show that it also returned when the
experiment ended. The findings are compared with mother–infant
experiments where evidence of interactional reciprocity has also
been found.
Ainsworth, Mary D. Salter, and Silvia M. Bell. 1970. ‟Attachment,
Exploration, and Separation: Illustrated by the Behavior of
One-Year-Olds in a Strange
Situation.” Child
Development 41 (1): 49–67.
Bowlby, John. 1958. ‟The
Nature of the Child’s Tie to His
Mother.” International
Journal of
Psycho-Analysis 39: 350–373.
de Waal, Frans. 2019. Mama’s
Last Hug: Animal Emotions and What They Tell Us about
Ourselves. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Dixon, Suzanne Z., Michael Yogman, Edward Tronick, Lauren Adamson, Heidelise Als, and Berry Brazelton. 1981. ‟Early
Infant Social Interaction with Parents and
Strangers.” Journal of the
American Academy of Child
Psychiatry 20: 32–52.
Drew, Paul. 1998. ‟Complaints
about Transgressions and
Misconduct.” Research on
Language and Social
Interaction 31 (3–4): 295–325.
Garfinkel, Harold. 1967. Studies
in Ethnomethodology. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Goode, David. 2007. Playing
with My Dog Katie. An Ethnomethodological Study of Dog–Human
Interaction. West Lafayette: Pursue University Press.
Heffner, Henry E. and Rickey S. Heffner. 2008. ‟High-Frequency
Fearing.” In The
Senses: A Comprehensive Reference. Vol. 3:
Audition ed.
by Peter Dallos and Donata Oertel, 55–60. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Heritage, John. 1984. Garfinkel
and
Ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity.
Horn, Lisa, Ludwig Huber, and Friederike Range. 2013. ‟The
Importance of the Secure Base Effect for Domestic Dogs –
Evidence from a Manipulative Problem-Solving
Task.” PLoS
ONE 8(5), e65296. .
Itakura, Shoji. 2004. ‟Gaze-Following
and Joint Visual Attention in Nonhuman
Animals.” Japanese
Psychological
Research 46 (3): 216–226.
Larson, Greger, Elinor K. Karlsson, Angela Perri, Matthew T. Webster, Simon Y. W. Ho, Joris Peters, Peter W. Stahl, Philip J. Piper, Frode Lingaas, Merete Fredholm, Kenine E. Comstock, Jaime F. Modiano, Claude Schelling, Alexander I. Agoulnik, Peter A. Leegwater, Keith Dobney, Jean-Denis Vigne, Carles Vilà, Leif Andersson, and Kerstin Lindblad-Toh. 2012. ‟Rethinking
Dog Domestication by Integrating Genetics, Archeology, and
Biogeography.” PNAS 109 (23): 8878–8883.
Lindwall, Oskar, Gustav Lymer, and Jonas Ivarsson. 2016. ‟Epistemic
Status and the Recognizability of Social
Actions.” Discourse
Studies 18 (5): 500–525.
Lynch, Michael. 2007. ‟Foreword.” In Playing
with My Dog Katie. An Ethnomethodological Study of Dog–Human
Interaction by David Goode, xiii–xv. West Lafayette: Pursue University Press.
Lyons-Ruth, Karlen, Nadia Bruschweiler-Stern, Alexandra M. Harrison, Alexander C. Morgan, Jeremy P. Nahum, Louis Sander, Daniel N. Stern, and Edward Z. Tronick. 1998. ‟Implicit
Relational Knowing: Its Role in Development and
Psychoanalytic
Treatment.” Infant Mental
Health
Journal 19 (3): 282–289.
Mesman, Judi, Marinus H. van I Jzendoorn, and Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg. 2009. ‟The
Many Faces of the Still-Face Paradigm: A Review and
Meta-Analysis.” Developmental
Review 29 (2): 120–162.
Mondada, Lorenza.2018a. ‟Multiple
Temporalities of Language and Body in Interaction:
Challenges for Transcribing
Multimodality.” Research on
Language and Social
Interaction 51 (1): 85–106.
Mondada, Lorenza.2018b. ‟The
Multimodal Interactional Organization of Tasting: Practices
of Tasting Cheese in Gourmet
Shops.” Discourse
Studies 20 (6): 743–769.
Murray, Lynne, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 1985. ‟Emotional
Regulation of Interactions Between Two Month-Olds and Their
Mothers.” In Social
Perception in Infants ed.
by Tiffany M. Field and Nathan A Fox, 177–197. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers.
Naderi, Szima, Ádám Miklósi, Dóka Antal, and Vilmos Csányi. 2001. ‟Co-Operative
Interactions between Blind Persons and Their
Dogs.” Applied Animal
Behaviour Science
74/2001: 59–80.
Nagasawa, Miho, Shouhei Mitsui, Shiori En, Nobuyo Ohtani, Mitsuaki Ohta, Yasuo Sakuma, Tatsushi Onaka, Kazutaka Mogi, and Takefumi Kikusui. 2015. ‟Oxytocin-Gaze
Positive Loop and the Coevolution of Human–Dog
Bonds.” Science 348 (6232): 333–336.
Ogden, Richard. 2010. ‟Prosodic
Constructions in Making
Complaint.” In Prosody
in Interaction ed.
by Dagmar Barth-Weingarten, Elisabeth Reber, and Margret Selting, 81–103. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Prato-Previde, Emanuela, and Paola Valsecchi2014. ‟The
Immaterial Cord: The Dog–Human Attachment
Bond.” In The
Social Dog. Behaviour and
Cognition ed.
by Juliane Kaminski and Sarah Marshall-Pescini, 165–189. Amsterdam: Academic Press.
Rehn, Therese, Ragen T. S. McGowan, and Linda J. Keeling2013. “Evaluating
the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) to Assess the Bond
between Dogs and
Humans.” PLoS
ONE 8 (2): e56938. .
Riede, Tobias, and Tecumseh Fitch1999. ‟Vocal
Tract Length and Acoustics of Vocalization in the Comestic
Dog (Canis
familiaris).”Journal of
Experimental
Biology 202: 2859–2867.
Sacks, Harvey, Emanuel A. Schegloff and Gail Jefferson1974. ‟A
Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for
Conversation”Language 50 (4): 696–735.
Sales, Gillian, and David Pye1974. Ultrasonic
Communication by
Animals. London: Chapman and Hall.
Schegloff, Emanuel A.2007. Sequence
Organization in Interaction. A Primer in Conversation
Analysis
I. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Selting, Margret. 2010. ‟Prosody
in Interaction: State of the
Art.” In Prosody
in Interaction ed.
by Dagmar Barth-Weingarten, Elisabeth Reber, and Margret Selting, 3–40. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Sidnell, Jack, and Tanya Stivers. eds.2013. The
Handbook of Conversation
Analysis. Malden: Blackwell.
Stern, Daniel N. 1971. ‟A
Micro-Analysis of Mother–Infant
Interaction.” Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 10 (3): 501–517.
Stivers, Tanya.2013. ‟Sequence
Organization.‟ In The
Handbook of Conversation Analysis, ed.
by Jack Sidnell and Tanya Stivers, 191–209. Malden: Blackwell.
Stivers, Tanya, and Federico Rossano. 2010. ‟Mobilizing
Response.” Research on
Language and Social
Interaction 43 (1): 3–31.
Stivers, Tanya, and Jack Sidnell. 2013. “Introduction.” In The
Handbook of Conversation Analysis, ed.
by Jack Sidnell and Tanya Stivers, 1–8. Malden: Blackwell.
Szczepek Reed, Beatrice. 2006. Prosodic
Orientation in English
Conversation. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.
Szczepek Reed, Beatrice. 2011. Analysing
Conversation. An Introduction to
Prosody. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan.
Taylor, Anna Magdalena, Victoria Frances Ratcliffe, Karen McComb, and David Reby. 2014. ‟Auditory
Communication in Domestic Dogs: Vocal Signalling in the
Extended Social Environment of a Companion
Animal.” In The
Social Dog. Behaviour and
Cognition, ed.
by Juliane Kaminski and Sarah Marshall-Pescini, 131–163. Amsterdam: Academic Press.
Tiira, Katriina, and Hannes Lohi. 2014. ‟Reliability
and Validity of a Questionnaire Survey in Canine Anxiety
Research.” Applied Animal
Behaviour
Science 155: 82–92.
Tomasello, Michael. 1995. ‟Joint
Attention as Social
Cognition.” In Joint
Attention: Its Origins and Role in
Development, ed.
by Chris Moore and Philip J. Dunham, 103–130. New York: Psychology Press.
Topál, József, and Márta Gácsi. 2012. ‟Lessons
We Should Learn from Our Unique Relationship with Dogs: An
Ethological
Approach.” In Crossing
Boundaries. Investigating Human–Animal
Relationships, ed.
by Lynda Birke and Jo Hockenhull, 163–186. Leiden: Brill.
Tronick, Edward, Heidelise Als, Lauren Adamson, Susan Wise, and Berry T. Brazelton. 1978. ‟The
Infant’s Response to Entrapment between Contradictory
Messages in Face-to-Face
Interaction.” Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 17 (1): 1–13.
Volodina, Elena V., Ilya A. Volodin, and Olga A. FilatovA2006. ‟The
Occurrence of Nonlinear Vocal Phenomena in Frustration
Whines of the Domestic Dog (Canis
familiaris).” In Advances
in Bioacoustics 2, Dissertationes Classis IV: Historia
Naturalis, ed.
by Matija Gogala and Tomi Trilar. Slovenian Academy of Sciences and Arts (Ljubljana), XLVII-3, 257–270.
Walker, Gareth. 2013. ‟Phonetics
and Prosody in
Conversation.‟ In The
Handbook of Conversation Analysis, ed.
by Jack Sidnell and Tanya Stivers, 455–474. Malden: Blackwell.
2024. Intersubjectivity as an analytical concept to study human-animal interaction in historical context: street dogs in Late Ottoman period. Frontiers in Sociology 9
Mondémé, Chloé
2023. Gaze in Interspecies Human–Pet Interaction: Some Exploratory Analyses. Research on Language and Social Interaction 56:4 ► pp. 291 ff.
Reed, Beatrice Szczepek
2023. Designing Talk for Humans and Horses: Prosody as a Resource for Parallel Recipient Design. Research on Language and Social Interaction 56:2 ► pp. 89 ff.
Harjunpää, Katariina
2022. Repetition and prosodic matching in responding to pets’ vocalizations. Langage et société N° 176:2 ► pp. 69 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.