Article In:
Pragmatics & Cognition
Vol. 31:1 (2024) ► pp.186205
References (41)
References
Arabmofrad, Ali., & Hamideh Marefat. 2008. Relative clause attachment ambiguity resolution in Persian. Iranian Journal of Applied Linguistics 11(1). 20–49.Google Scholar
Ariel, Mira. 1990. Accessing noun-phrase antecedents (RLE Linguistics B: Grammar). Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2001. Accessibility theory: An overview. In Ted J. M. Sanders, Joost Schilperoord & Wilbert Spooren (eds.), Text representation: Linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers, & Harry J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3). 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ben-Shachar, Matten S., Daniel Lüdecke & Do minique Makowski. 2020. Effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. Journal of Open Source Software 5(56). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cheng, Yesi, Jason Rothman & Ian Cunnings. 2021. Parsing preferences and individual differences in nonnative sentence processing: Evidence from eye movements. Applied Psycholinguistics 42(1). 129–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cho, Jacee. 2017. The acquisition of different types of definite noun phrases in L2-English. International Journal of Bilingualism 21(3). 367–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, Harald & Claudia Felser. 2006. Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 27(1). 3–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2018. Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40(3). 693–706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clifton Jr. Charles, & Fernanda Ferreira. 1989. Ambiguity in context. Language and Cognitive Processes 4(3–4). SI77–SI103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crain, Stephen & Mark Steedman. 1985. On not being led up the garden path: The use of context by the psychological syntax processor. In David R. Dowty, Lauri Karttunen, & Arnold M. Zwicky (eds.), Natural language parsing: Psychological, computational and theoretical perspectives, 320–358. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuetos, Fernando & Don C. Mitchell. 1988. Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition 30(1). 73–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cunnings, Ian. 2017. Parsing and working memory in bilingual sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 20(4). 659–678. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, Laurent, Bryan Donaldson, Amanda C. Edmonds, Audrey Liljestrand Fultz & Rebecca A. Petrush. 2008. Syntactic and prosodic computations in the resolution of relative clause attachment ambiguity by English-French learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 30(4). 453–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Epstein, Richard. 2002. The definite article, accessibility, and the construction of discourse referents. Cognitive Linguistics 12(4). 333–378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Felser, Claudia. 2019. Structure-sensitive constraints in non-native sentence processing. Journal of the European Second Language Association 3(1). 12–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernández, Eva M. 1999. Processing strategies in second language acquisition: Some preliminary results. In Elaine C. Klein & Gita Martohardjono (eds.), The development of second language grammars: A generative approach, 217–240. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, Kenneth I., & Jonathan C. Forster. 2003. DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behaviour Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 35(1).116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn. 1978. On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Connecticut: University of Connecticut PhD dissertation.
Frazier, Lyn & Charles Clifton. 1996. Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, Cheryl. 1997. Examining second language reading: An on-line look. In Antonella Sorace, Caroline B. Heycock & Richard Shillcock (eds.) Proceedings of the GALA ’97 Conference on Language Acquisition, 444–449. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.Google Scholar
. 2002. An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language. In Roberto R. Heredia & Jeanette Altarriba (eds.), Bilingual sentence processing, 217–236. Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, Edward, Neal J. Pearlmutter, Enriqueta Canseco-González & Gregory Hickok. 1996. Recency preferences in the human sentence processing mechanism. Cognition 59(1). 23–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Givón, Thomas. 1992. The grammar of referential coherence as mental processing instructions. Linguistics 30(1). 5–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goad, Heather., Natalia B. Guzzo & Lydia White. 2021. Parsing ambiguous relative clauses in L2 English: Learner sensitivity to prosodic cues. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 43(1). 83–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heim, Irene Roswitha. 1982. The semantics of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Massachusetts: University of Massachusetts PhD dissertation.
Hopp, Holger. 2006. Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research 22(3). 369–397. [URL]
. 2014. Working memory effects in the L2 processing of ambiguous relative clauses. Language Acquisition 21(3). 250–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ishihara, Kenji., Elizabeth Hiser & Tae Okada. 2003. Modifying C-Test for practical purposes. Doshisha Studies in Language and Culture (4). 539–568.Google Scholar
Klein, Natalie M., Whitney Gegg-Harrison, Greg N. Carlson & Michael K. Tanenhaus. 2013. Experimental investigations of weak definite and weak indefinite noun phrases. Cognition 128(2). 187–213. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per B. Brockhoff & Rune H. B. Christensen. 2017. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82(13). 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pan, Hui-Yu, Sarah Schimke & Claudia Felser. 2015. Referential context effects in non-native relative clause ambiguity resolution. International Journal of Bilingualism 19(3). 298–313. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papadopoulou, Despina & Harald Clahsen. 2003. Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25(4). 501–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. 2020. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [URL]
Solaimani, Ehsan, Florence Myles & Laurel Lawyer. 2023. Is L2 parsing qualitatively different from native parsing? The role of context, L1, and individual differences in working memory capacity and proficiency [Manuscript submitted for publication]. Department of Language and Linguistics, University of Essex.Google Scholar
Spivey, Michael J. & Michael K. Tanenhaus. 1998. Syntactic ambiguity resolution in discourse: Modelling the effects of referential context and lexical frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24(6). 1521–1543. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trace, Jonathan. 2020. Clozing the gap: How far do cloze items measure? Language Testing 37(2). 235–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, Roger P. G., Martin J. Pickering, Matthew J. Traxler. 2000. Unrestricted race: A new model of syntactic ambiguity resolution. In Allan Kennedy, Ralph Radach, Dieter Heller & Joel Pynte (eds.), Reading as a perceptual process, 621–648. Amsterdam: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Warren, Tessa & Edward Gibson. 2002. The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition 85 (1). 79–112. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zehr, Jeremy & Florian Schwarz. 2018. Penncontroller for internet-based experiments (ibex). DOI logoGoogle Scholar