Review published In:
Pragmatics & Cognition
Vol. 26:2/3 (2019) ► pp.474481
References
Bar-Hillel, Joshua
1971Out of the pragmatic wastebasket. Linguistic Inquiry 21. 401–407.Google Scholar
Beebe, James R.
(ed.) 2014Advances in experimental epistemology. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Bosco, Francesca M., Maurizio Tirassa & Ilaria Gabbatore
2018Why pragmatics and Theory of Mind do not (completely) overlap. Frontiers in Psychology 91. 1453. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H.
1975Bridging. In Roger C. Schank & Brian L. Nash-Webber (eds.), Theoretical issues in natural language processing. New York: Association for Computing Machinery, 169–174.Google Scholar
Green, Judith
1973Psycholinguistics: Chomsky and psychology. Harmondsworth, U.K.: Penguin.Google Scholar
Grice, Paul
1976Logic and conversation. In Peter Cole & Jerry L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and Semantics, vol. 3, Speech Acts, 41–58. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
1989Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Győri, Miklos
2006Autism and cognitive architecture: Domain specificity and cognitive theorizing on autism. Budapest: Akadémiai.Google Scholar
Kagel, John H. & Alvin E. Roth
(eds.) 1997/2016The handbook of experimental economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Knobe, Joshua & Shaun Nichols
2014Experimental philosophy, vol. 21. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Labov, William & David Fanshel
1977Therapeutic discourse: Psychotherapy as conversation. New York, N. Y.: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C.
2000Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luria, Alexandr R.
1971Towards the problem of the historical nature of psychological processes. International Journal of Psychology 61. 259–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luria, Alexandr R. & Lev S. Vygotsky
1992Ape, primitive man, and the child: Essays in the history of behavior. New York, N.Y.: Harvester.Google Scholar
Marr, David
1982Vision. San Francisco, Ca: Freeman.Google Scholar
McDonald, Skye
2013Impairments in social cognition following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 191. 231–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A.
2001When children are more logical than adults: Experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 781. 165–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A. & Annette Reboul
2008Experimental pragmatics: A Gricean turn in the study of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 121. 425–431. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira & Dan Sperber
(eds.) 2004Experimental pragmatics. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noveck, Ira A. & Dan Sperber
2007The why and how of experimental pragmatics: The case of “scalar inferences”. In Noel Burton-Roberts (ed.), Advances in Pragmatics, 184–212. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sacks, Harvy, Emmanuel A. Schegloff & Gail Jefferson
1974A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 501. 696–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, Emmanuel A.
2000Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language in Society 291. 1–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scribner, Sylvia
1997Mind and Social Practice: Selected Writings of Sylvia Scribner. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan & Deirdre Wilson
1995Relevance: Communication and Cognition, 2nd edn. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Tiel, Bob van, Ira Noveck & Mikhail Kissine
2018Reasoning with ‘some’. Journal of Semantics 351. 757–797.Google Scholar