Article published in:
Diagrammatic Reasoning
Edited by Riccardo Fusaroli and Kristian Tylén
[Pragmatics & Cognition 22:2] 2014
► pp. 264283
References

References

Alibali, M.W., Spencer, R.C., Knox, L., & Kita, S.
(2011) Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychol Sci, 22(9), 1138–1144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Atã, P., Bitarello, B., & Queiroz, J.
(2014) Iconic semiosis and representational efficiency in the London Underground Diagram. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(2), 177–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A.
(2012) Diagramming phenomena for mechanistic explanation. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Bjørndahl, J., Fusaroli, R., Østergaard, S., & Tylén, K.
(2014) Thinking together with material representations: Joint epistemic actions in creative problem solving. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(1), 103–123.Google Scholar
2015). When agreeing is not enough. Interaction Studies, 16(3).
Bjørndahl, J.S., Fusaroli, R., Østergaard, S., & Tylén, K.
(2014) Thinking together with material representations: Joint epistemic actions in creative problem solving. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(1), 103–123.Google Scholar
Boysen, S.T., Bernston, G.G., Hannan, M.B., & Cacioppo, J.T.
(1996) Quantity-based interference and symbolic representations in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of experimental psychology . Animal behavior processes, 22(1), 76–76. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burnston, D., Sheredos, B., Abrahamsen, A., & Bechtel, W.
(2015) Scientists’ Use of Diagrams in Developing Mechanistic Explanations, Pragmatics and Cognition. Pragmatics and Cognition., 22(2), 224–242 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cairo, A.
(2012) The Functional Art: An introduction to information graphics and visualization. New Riders.Google Scholar
Clark, A.
(2006a) Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends Cogn Sci, 10(8), 370–374. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Material symbols. Philosophical psychology, 19(3), 291–307. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A., & Chalmers, D.
(1998) The extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & Krych, M.A.
(2004) Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding. 62–81.Google Scholar
Cowley, S.J., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. d. r
(Eds.) (2014) Cognition Beyond the Brain: Computation, Interactivity and Human Artifice (Vol. 17). Springer.Google Scholar
Dale, R., Fusaroli, R., Duran, N., & Richardson, D.C.
(2013) The self-organization of human interaction. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 59, 43–95. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, J.A., Stephen, D.G., Boncoddo, R., & Anastas, J.
(2010) The self-organization of cognitive structure. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 52, 343–384. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duncker, K.
(1945) The Structure and Dynamics of Problem-Solving Processes. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), 1–112. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M.
(2002) The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fay, N., Garrod, S., Roberts, L., & Swoboda, N.
(2010) The interactive evolution of human communication systems. Cogn Sci, 34(3), 351–386. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Rees, G., Frith, C.D., Roepstorff, A., & Tylén, K.
(2012) Coming to terms: an experimental quantification of the coordinative benefits of linguistic interaction. Psychological Science, 23, 931–939. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Gangopadhyay, N., & Tylén, K.
(2014) The dialogically extended mind: Making a case for language as skilful intersubjective engagement. Cognitive Systems Research, 29-30, 31–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K.
(2014) Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 147–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K.
(2012) Carving Language for Social Coordination: a dynamic approach. Interaction Studies, 13, 103–123. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
in press). Investigating conversational dynamics: Interactive alignment, Interpersonal synergy, and collective task performance. Cognitive Science.
Galantucci, B., & Garrod, S.
(2010) A new approach for studying the emergence and the evolution of human communication. Interaction studies, 11(1), 1–13. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Doherty, G.
(1994) Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions. Cognition, 53(3), 181–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., Fay, N., Lee, J., Oberlander, J., & MacLeod, T.
(2007) Foundations of Representation: Where Might Graphical Symbol Systems Come From? Cognitive Science, 31(6), 961–987. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grant, E.R., & Spivey, M.J.
(2003) Eye movements and problem solving guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14(5), 462–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Healey, P.G., Swoboda, N., Umata, I., & King, J.
(2007) Graphical language games: Interactional constraints on representational form. Cognitive Science, 31(2), 285–309.Google Scholar
Hintikka, J.
(1983) The game of language: Studies in game-theoretical semantics and its applications. Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2007) Socratic Epistemology: Explorations of Knowledge-Seeking by Questioning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E.
(1995) Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1555–1577. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Cognitive Ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 705–715. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1995) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. MIT press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P.
(1994) On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18(4), 513–549. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D.
(1999) Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 1534. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W.
(1925) Komplextheorie und Gestalttheorie. Psychological Research, 6(1), 358–416. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
(1999) Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Maglio, P.P., Matlock, T., Raphaely, D., Chernicky, B., & Kirsh, D.
(1999) Interactive skill in scrabble. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Ohlsson, S.
(1992) Information-processing explanations of insight and related phenomena. Advances in the psychology of thinking, chap. 1, 1–44.Google Scholar
Peirce, C.S.
(1931) Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce: Vols. 1-6 (C. Hartshorne & P. Weiss, eds.). Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
(1976) The New Elements of Mathematics by C.S. Peirce, edited by Carolyn Eisele. Walter De Gruyter.Google Scholar
(1981) Writings of Charles S. Peirce : a chronological edition. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
(1998) Charles S. Peirce : the essential writings. Amherst, N.Y.: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Schwartz, D.L.
(1995) The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4(3), 321–354. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sheredos, B., Burnston, D., Abrahamsen, A., & Bechtel, W.
(2013) Why do biologists use so many diagrams? Philosophy of Science, 80(5), 931–944. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shin, S.-J.
(2010) Peirce’s two ways of abstraction. In M. Moore (Ed.), New Essays on Peirce’s Mathematical Philosophy. Chicago and La Salle: Open Court.Google Scholar
Stjernfelt, F.
(2000) Diagrams as centerpiece of a Peircean epistemology. Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society, XXXVI(3), 357–392.Google Scholar
(2007) Diagrammatology: An investigation on the borderlines of phenomenology, ontology, and semiotics. New York: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Natural Propositions: The Actuality of Peirce’s Doctrine of Dicisigns. Docent Press.Google Scholar
Tek, S., Mesite, L., Fein, D., & Naigles, L.
(2014) Longitudinal analyses of expressive language development reveal two distinct language profiles among young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 44(1), 75–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tylén, K., Fusaroli, R., Bundgaard, P., & Østergaard, S.
(2013) Making sense together: A dynamical account of linguistic meaning making. Semiotica, 194, 39–62.Google Scholar
Vallée-Tourangeau, F.
(2013) Interactivity, efficiency, and individual differences in mental arithmetic. Experimental psychology, 60(4), 302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Weller, A., Villejoubert, G., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F.
(2011) Interactive insight problem solving. Thinking & Reasoning, 17(4), 424–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yau, N.
(2011) Visualize this: the FlowingData guide to design, visualization, and statistics. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 10 other publications

Christensen, Peer, Riccardo Fusaroli & Kristian Tylén
2016. Environmental constraints shaping constituent order in emerging communication systems: Structural iconicity, interactive alignment and conventionalization. Cognition 146  pp. 67 ff. Crossref logo
Fabry, Regina E. & Markus Pantsar
2021. A fresh look at research strategies in computational cognitive science: The case of enculturated mathematical problem solving. Synthese 198:4  pp. 3221 ff. Crossref logo
Fontainha, Tharcisio Cotta, Adriana Leiras, Renata Albergaria de Mello Bandeira & Luiz Felipe Scavarda
2017. Public-Private-People Relationship Stakeholder Model for disaster and humanitarian operations. International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 22  pp. 371 ff. Crossref logo
Hohol, Mateusz & Marcin Miłkowski
2019. Cognitive Artifacts for Geometric Reasoning. Foundations of Science 24:4  pp. 657 ff. Crossref logo
Marshall, Guy Clarke & André Freitas
2021. Measuring diagram quality through semiotic morphisms. Semiotica 2021:239  pp. 125 ff. Crossref logo
Omoge, Michael
2021. Naturalised modal epistemology and quasi-realism. South African Journal of Philosophy 40:3  pp. 229 ff. Crossref logo
Philipsen, Johanne S. & Sarah Bro Trasmundi
2019. Gesture reuse as distributed embodied cognition. Gesture 18:1  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Reid, Heleen A. J., Clare Hocking & Liz Smythe
2019. The making of occupation-based models and diagrams: History and semiotic analysis. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy 86:4  pp. 313 ff. Crossref logo
Stege Bjørndahl, Johanne, Riccardo Fusaroli, Svend østergaard & Kristian Tylén
2015. Agreeing is not enough. Interaction Studies. Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems 16:3  pp. 495 ff. Crossref logo
Tylén, Kristian, Riccardo Fusaroli, Sergio Rojo, Katrin Heimann, Nicolas Fay, Niels N. Johannsen, Felix Riede & Marlize Lombard
2020. The evolution of early symbolic behavior inHomo sapiens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117:9  pp. 4578 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 09 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.