Article published In:
Pragmatics & Cognition
Vol. 30:1 (2023) ► pp.92119
References
Bach, Kent
1999The myth of conventional implicature. Linguistics and Philosophy 221. 327–366. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barlaz, Marissa
2022Ordinal logistic regression in R. [URL] (Last access: 18 November 2022)
Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers & Harry J. Tily
2013Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 681. 255–278. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borst, Jelmer P., Niels A. Taatgen & Hedderik van Rij
2010The problem state: A cognitive bottleneck in multitasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, Cognition 36(2). 363–382. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barton, Stephen B. & Anthony J. Sanford
1993A case study of anomaly detection: Shallow semantic processing and cohesion establishment. Memory & Cognition 21(4). 477–487. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brauer, Markus & John J. Curtin
2018Linear mixed-effects models and the analysis of non-independent data: A unified framework to analyze categorical and continuous independent variables that vary within-subjects and/or within items. Psychological Methods 23(3). 389–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brocca, Nicola, Davide Garassino & Viviana Masia
2016Politici nella rete o nella rete dei politici? L’implicito nella comunicazione politica italiana su Twitter. PhiN-Beiheft 111. 68–79.Google Scholar
Burkhardt, Petra
2007The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. NeuroReport 18(17). 1851–1854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace
1994Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Christensen, Rune Haubo B.
2019Ordinal-regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019, 12–10. Available online at: [URL] (Last access: 4 December 2021).
Christiansen, Morten H. & Nick Chater
2016The now-or-never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 391. e62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, Louis
2013Background relevance. Journal of Pragmatics 591. 178–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, Louis & Steve Oswald
2009Argumentation et engagement du locuteur: Pour un point de vue subjectiviste. Nouveaux cahiers de linguistique française 291. 215–243.Google Scholar
Domaneschi, Filippo & Di Paola, Simona
2018The Processing Costs of Presupposition Accommodation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 47(3):483–503. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domaneschi, Filippo, Elena Carrea, Carlo Penco & Alberto Greco
2014The cognitive load of presupposition triggers: Mandatory and optional repairs in presupposition failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 291. 136–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domaneschi, Filippo, Paolo Canal, Viviana Masia, Edoardo Lombardi Vallauri & Valentina Bambini
2018N400 and P600 modulation in presupposition accommodation: The effect of different trigger types. Journal of Neurolinguistics 451. 13–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Drai, Nathanaël & Louis de Saussure
2016Quand l’implicite devient explicite: D’un accident expérimental à une étude pilote. Syntaxe et Sémantique 2016/1(17). 115–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Endresen, Anna & Laura A. Janda
2016Five statistical models for Likert-type experimental data on acceptability judgments. Journal of Research Design and Statistics in Linguistics and Communication Science 3(2). 217–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda & Matthew W. Lowder
2016Prediction, information structure, and good-enough language processing. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 651. 217–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garassino, Davide, Viviana Masia & Nicola Brocca
2019Tweet as you speak. The role of implicit strategies and pragmatic functions in political communication: Data from a diamesic comparison. Rassegna Italiana di Linguistica Applicata 2–31. 187–208.Google Scholar
Garassino, Davide, Nicola Brocca & Viviana Masia
2022Is implicit communication quantifiable? A corpus-based analysis of British and Italian political tweets. Journal of Pragmatics 1941. 9–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan T.
2020On classification trees and random forests in corpus linguistics: Some words of caution and suggestions for improvement. Corpus Linguistics & Linguistic Theory 16(3). 617–647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hothorn, Torsten, Kurt Hornik & Achim Zeileis
2006Unbiased recursive partitioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics 15(3). 651–674. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, Stephen
1983Pragmatics. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loftus, Elizabeth F.
1975Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology 7(4). 560–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lombardi Vallauri, Edoardo
2016The “exaptation” of linguistic implicit strategies. Springer Plus 51. 1106. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2019La lingua disonesta. Roma: Carocci.Google Scholar
Lombardi Vallauri, Edoardo & Viviana Masia
2014Implicitness impact: Measuring texts. Journal of Pragmatics 611. 161–184. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lombardi Vallauri, Edoardo & Viviana Masia
2015Facilitating automation in sentence processing: The emergence of topic and presupposition in human communication. Topoi 37(2). 343–354. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lowder, Matthew W. & Peter C. Gordon
2015Focus takes time: Structural effects on reading. Psychonomic Bulletin Review 221. 1733–1738. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lucisano, Pietro & Maria E. Piemontese
1988GULPEASE: Una formula per la predizione della difficoltà dei testi in lingua italiana. Scuola e città XXXIX (3). 110–124.Google Scholar
Lüdecke, Daniel
2018ggeffects: Tidy data frames of marginal effects from regression models. Journal of Open Source Software 3(26). 772. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lyons, John
1977Semantics. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirsten, Mareike, Sonja Tiemann, Verena C. Seibold, Ingo Hertrich, Sigrid Beck & Bettina Rolke
2014When the polar bear encounters many polar bears: Event-related potential context effects evoked by uniqueness failure. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(9). 1147–1162. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Masia, Viviana, Paolo Canal, Irene Ricci, Edoardo Lombardi Vallauri & Valentina Bambini
2017Presupposition of new information as a pragmatic garden path: Evidence from Event-related brain potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics 421. 31–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morency, Patrick, Steve Oswald & Louis de Saussure
2008Explicitness, implicitness and commitment attribution: A cognitive pragmatic approach. Belgian Journal of Linguistics 221. 197–219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, Misha L.
2018Accommodation: A cognitive heuristic for background information. Anglophonia 251. Available online at: [URL]. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piciucco, Emanuela, Viviana Masia, Emanuele Maiorana, Edoardo Lombardi Vallauri & Patrizio Campisi
2022Information structure effects on the processing of nouns and verbs: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognition 14(1). 85–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven, Martin A. Nowak & James J. Lee
2008The logic of indirect speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(3). 833–838. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
2022R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available online at: [URL] (Last access: 24 May 2023).
Sbisà, Marina
2007Pathways to explicitness. Lingue e Linguaggio 61. 101–120.Google Scholar
Schwarz, Florian
2015Presuppositions vs. asserted content in online processing. In Florian Schwarz (ed.), Experimental perspectives on presupposition: Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics, 89–108. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan & Deirdre Wilson
1995Relevance: Communication and cognition, 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan, Fabrice Clément, Cristophe Heintz, Olivier Mascaro, Hugo Mercier, Gloria Origgi & Deirdre Wilson
2010Epistemic vigilance. Mind and Language 25(4). 359–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stalnaker, Robert
1973Presuppositions. Journal of Philosophical Logic 2(4). 447–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2002Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy 251. 701–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tiemann, Sonja
2014The processing of wieder (“again”) and other presupposition triggers. Tübingen: Universität Tübingen PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Tiemann, Sonja, Mareike Schmid, Nadine Bade, Bettina Rolke, Ingo Hertrich, Herman Ackermann, Julia Knapp & Sigrid Beck
2011Psycholinguistic evidence for presuppositions: On-line and off-line data. In Ingo Reich, Eva Horch & Dennis Pauly (eds.), Proceedings of Sinn & Bedeutung 15 1, 581–595. Saarbrucken: Saarland University Press.Google Scholar
von Fintel, Kai
2008What is presupposition accommodation, again? Philosophical Perspectives 22(1). 137–170. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wickham, Hadley et al.
2019Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43). 1686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Deirdre
1975Presuppositions and non-truth-conditional semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT dissertation PhD dissertation.
Wilson, Deirdre & Dan Sperber
1979Ordered entailments: An alternative to presuppositional theories. Syntax and Semantics 111. 299–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1981/1979On Grice’s theory of conversation. In Paul Werth (ed.), Conversation and discourse, 155–178. London: Routledge.Google Scholar