Commentary published In:
Pedagogical Linguistics
Vol. 3:2 (2022) ► pp.174180
References
DeKeyser, R. M.
(1995) Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17 (3), 379–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C.
(2005) At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 (2), 305–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R.
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27 (2), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S.
(1997) Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gilquin, G.
(2021) Using corpora to foster L2 construction learning: A data-driven learning experiment. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 31 (2), 229–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A.
(2004) Extending collostructional analysis. A corpus-based perspective on ‘alternations’. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 9 (1), 97–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hirata, Y., & Thompson, P.
(2021) Communicative data-driven learning: a two-year pilot study. ELT Journal. DOI logo, in press .Google Scholar
Jäschke, K., & Plag, I.
(2016) The dative alternation in German-English interlanguage. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38 (3), 485–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jenset, G. B., McGillivray, B., & Rundell, M.
(2018) The dative alternation revisited. Fresh insights from contemporary British spoken data. In V. Brezina, R. Love, & K. Aijmer (Eds.), Corpus approaches to contemporary British speech. Sociolinguistic studies of the spoken BNC2014 (pp. 185–208). Oxon and New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar. Volume 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D.
(2003) Teaching language: From grammar to grammaring. Boston, MA: Heinle.Google Scholar
Lin, M. H.
(2021) Effects of data-driven learning on college students of different grammar proficiencies: A preliminary empirical assessment in EFL classes. SAGE Open Journal, online, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lo, M. L., & Marton, F.
(2012) Towards a science of the art of teaching: Using variation theory as a guiding principle of pedagogical design. International Journal for Lesson and Learning Studies, 1 (1), 7–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Llopis-García, R.
Madlener, K.
(2015) Frequency effects in instructed second language acquisition. Applications of Cognitive Linguistics, Volume 291. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nassaji, H.
(2017) Grammar acquisition. In S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of instructed language acquisition (pp. 205–223). New York: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ott, G.
(2017) Exploring Variation Theory in form-focused language teaching. Teaching the present perfect in upper secondary EFL. CELT Matters (Centre for English Language Teaching), 11, 9–29. [URL]
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T.
(2002) The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6 (11), 456–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N.
(2012) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33 (4), 829–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B.
(1993) Grammar teaching for the acquisition-rich classroom. Foreign Language Annals, 26 (4), 435–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, M. J., & Ionin, T.
(2021) Does explicit instruction affect L2 linguistic competence? An examination with L2 acquisition of English inverse scope. Second Language Research, 1 (31), online. DOI logoGoogle Scholar