Article published In:
Pragmatics
Vol. 10:4 (2000) ► pp.415438
References
Baron-Cohen, S., H. Tager-Flusberg, and D.J. Cohen
(eds.) (2000) Understanding other minds. Second edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Black, J.B., and G.H. Bower
(1980) Story understanding as problem solving. Poetics 91: 223–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, CM
., and P. Haggort (ed.) (1999) The neurocognition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P., and P. Ricoeur
(1998) La nature et la règle. Paris: Odile Jacob.Google Scholar
Chantraine, Y., Y. Joanette, and D. Carbedat
(1998) Impairments of discourse-level representation and processes. In B. Stemmer and H.A. Whittaker (eds.), Handbook of neurolinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 261–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, A.
(1989) Microcognition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1994) Associative engines. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H., and E. Clark
(1977) Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt.  BoPGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., and S.E. Haviland
(1977) Comprehension and the given-new contract. In R.O. Freedle (ed.), Discourse production and comprehension. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex, pp. 1–40.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M.
(1999) Modularity and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 31: 115–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L., and J. Tooby
(1992) Psychological foundations of culture. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (eds.), (1992) The adapted mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crozier, S., A. Sirigu, S. Lehéricy, P.-F. van de Moortele, B. Pillon, J. Grafman, Y. Agid, B. Dubois, and D. Le Bihan
(1999) Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: An FMRI study. Neuropsychologia 371: 1469–1476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., and Gy. Gergely
(1998) The ideological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science 11: 255–259. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G, Gy. Gergely, S. Biro, S. Koos, and M. Brockbank
(1999) Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of’pure reason’ in infancy. Cognition 721: 237–267. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D.
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1990) The interpretation of texts, people and other artifacts. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 1, Supplement pp. 177–194. DOI logo
(1991) Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown ss: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D., and M. Kinsbourne
(1992) Time and the observer. The where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 151: 183–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J.
(2000) Language and theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, and D.J. Cohen (eds.), Understanding other minds. Second edition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 83–123.Google Scholar
Dressler, W.U., and Cs. Pléh
(1988) On text disturbances in aphasia. In W.U. Dressier and J.A. Stark (eds.), Linguistic analyses of aphasie language. New York: Springer, pp. 151–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressier, W.U., and H. Stark
(2000) Clinical impairments of text pragmatics: Linguistic or cognitive? Paper presented at the 7th International Pragmatics Conference, Budapest, July 10th, 2000.
Dressler, W.U., R. Wodak, and Cs. Pléh
(1990) Gender-specific discourse differences in aphasia. In Y. Joanette and H.H. Brownell (eds.), Discourse ability and brain damage. New York: Springer, pp. 236–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressier. W.U., and Cs. Pléh
(1984) Zur narrative textkompetenz von Aphatikern. In W.U. Dressier, and R. Wodak (eds.), Patholinguistische Studien. Hamburg: Buschke, 1984.Google Scholar
Elman, J.L., E.A. Bates, M.H. Johnson, A. Karmiloff-Srnith, D. Parisi, and K. Plunkett
(1996) Rethinking innateness: A connectionistperspective on development. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J.A.
(1983) The modularity of mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J.
(2000) The mind doesn ‘t work that way. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., and I. Olbrei
(1973) Semantic heuristics and syntactic analysis. Cognition 21: 319–347. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frith, U.
(1989) Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gergely, Gy
., and Cs. Pléh (1994) Lexical processing in an agglutinative language and the organization of the lexicon. Folia Linguistica 281: 175–204. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gergely Gyórgy, Z. Nâdasdy, G. Csibra, and S. Biró
(1995) Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 561: 165–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A.C.
(1992) Questions and Information Systems. Hillsdale, N.J.: L. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(1996) Models of Understanding Text. Mahlah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Graesser, A.C., and L.F. Clark
(1986) Structure and procedures of implicit knowledge. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Grice, P.
(1975) Logic and conversation. In P. Cole and J.L. Morgan (eds.), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3. Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Györi Miklós
(2000) Domain specificity and autism. Unpublished manuscript, Eötvös Lorând University, Budapest.
Györi, M., I. Kanizsai-Nagy, and Gy. K. Stefanik
(2000) Mentalization and linguistic pragmatics in well functioning autistic subjects. Talk presented at theVlllth Hungarian Cognitive Science Meeting, Szeged, February 4–6, 2000.
Halâsz, L., J. Laszló, and Cs. Pléh
(1988) Cross-cultural studies in reading short stories. Poetics 171: 287–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harnish, R.M.
(1994) The architectures involved in language. In M.R. Harnish (ed.), Philosophical issues in linguistics. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hirshfeld, L.A., and S.A. Gelman
(eds.) (1994.) Mapping the Mind. Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivaskó, L.
(2000) On the causes of aphasie disturbances. Talk presented at theVlllth Hungarian Cognitive Science Meeting, Szeged, February 4–6, 2000.
Jolliffe, T., and S. Baron-Cohen
(1999) A test of the central coherence theory in autism. Cognition 711: 149-185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1992) Beyond Modularity: A Developmental perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lange, F. A.
(1881) History of Materialism. I-III. London: Trübner.Google Scholar
Laszló, J.
(1986) Scripts for interpersonal situations. Studia Psychologica 281: 125–135.Google Scholar
Leslie, A.
(1987) Pretense and representation: The origins of the “theory of mind”. Psychological Review 941: 412–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luria, A.R.
(1961) The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
(1966) Higher Cortical Function in Man. New York : Basic Books.Google Scholar
(1969) Visshije korkovije funkcii cheloveka. 2nd edition. Moscow: Moscow University Press.Google Scholar
(1973) Osnovi nejropsihologii. Moscow: Moscow University Press.Google Scholar
(1974) Nejropsihologija pamjati. Vol. 1 Moscow: Pedagogika.Google Scholar
(1981) Language and cognition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
McDonald, S.
(1998) Communication and language disturbances following traumatic brain injury. In B. Stemmer and H.A. Whittaker (eds.), Handbook ofneurolinguistics. SanDiego: Academic Press, pp. 485–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S.
(1996) The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Müller, RA.
(1996) Innateness, autonomy, universality? Neurological approaches to language. Behavior and Brain Sciences 191: 611–675. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1991) Rules of language. Science 2531: 530–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., and A. Prince
(1994) Regular and irregular morphology and the psychological status of rules of grammar. In S.D. Lima, R.L. Corrigan, and G.K. Iverson (eds.), The reality of linguistic rules. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 321–350. DOI logo  BoPGoogle Scholar
Pléh, Cs.
(1985) Brain language. On a book by Jerry A. Fodor. Folia Linguistica 1985.191: 539–548.Google Scholar
Phylyshyn, Z.W.
(1984) Computation and cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reboul, A., and J. Moeschler
(1998a) Pragmatique du discours. Paris: Armand Colin.  BoPGoogle Scholar
(1998b) La pragmatuque aujourd’hui. Paris: Seuil.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D.E.
(1975) Notes on a schema for stories. In D.G. Bobrow and A.N. Collins (eds.), Representation and Understanding. New York: Academic Press, pp. 211–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, E.M., and A. Sholl
(1999) Clues to the functional and neural architecture of word meaning. In CM. Brown and P. Haggort (eds.), The neurocognition of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 241–272.Google Scholar
Schank, R., and R.P. Abelson
(1977) Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.  BoPGoogle Scholar
School, B.J., and P.D. Tremoulet
(2000) Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 41: 299–309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sirigu, A., T. Cohen, T. Zalla, P. Pradat-Diehl, P. van Eeckhout, and J. Grafman
(1998) Distinct frontal regions for processing sentence syntax and story grammar. Cortex 341: 771–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sirigu, A., T. Zalla, B. Pillon, J. Grafman, Y. Agid, and B. Dubois
(1996) Encoding of Sequence and bundaries of scripts following prefrontal lesions. Cortex 321: 297–310. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D.
(1996) Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.  BoPGoogle Scholar
(2000) Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2000) Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg and D.J. Cohen (eds.), Understanding other minds. Second edition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 124–149.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., G. Carlson, and J.C. Trueswell
(1989) The role of thematic structures in interpretation and parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes 41: 211–234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Thuma, O., and Cs. Pléh
(1995) Kétértelmuségek feldolgozâsa két nyelv között. (Ambiguity resolution between two languages) Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle 511:28-40.Google Scholar
(1999) Ambiguous data on Hungarian ambiguity resolution. In M. Prinzhorn and J. Rennison (eds.), Dressier Festschrift.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, Mass. : Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Van Lancker, D., and N.A. Pachana
(1998) The influence of emotions on language and communication disorders. In B. Stemmer and H.A. Whittaker (eds.), Handbook ofneurolinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 301–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wodak, R., W.U. Dressier, and Cs. Pléh
(1984) Geschlechtsspezifisches Sprachverhalten von Aphatikern auf der Textebene. In W.U. Dressler and R. Wodak (eds.), Patholinguistische Studien. Hamburg: Buschke.Google Scholar
Zalla, T, A. Sirigu, B. Pillon, B. Dubois, J. Grafman, and Y. Agid
(1998) Deficit in evaluating pre-determinated sequences of script events in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 341: 621–628. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zalla, T., A. Sirigu, B. Pillon, B. Dubois, Y. Agid, and J. Grafman
(in press) How patients with Parkinson’s disease retrieve and manage action knowledge? Cortex.
Cited by

Cited by 11 other publications

Forgács, Bálint, Ágnes Lukács & Csaba Pléh
2014. Lateralized processing of novel metaphors: Disentangling figurativeness and novelty. Neuropsychologia 56  pp. 101 ff. DOI logo
Forgács, Bálint & Csaba Pléh
2022. The Fluffy Metaphors of Climate Science. In Metaphors and Analogies in Sciences and Humanities [Synthese Library, 453],  pp. 447 ff. DOI logo
Győri, Miklós, Lukács, Ágnes & Pléh, Csaba
2004. Towards the Understanding of the Neurogenesis of Social Cognition: Evidence from Impaired Populations. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology 2:3  pp. 261 ff. DOI logo
Kiefer, Ferenc
2002. Wolfgang U. Dressler–Oskar E. Pfeiffer–Markus Pöchträger–John R. Rennison (eds): Morphological analysis in comparison. Current Issues in Linguistic Theory Vol. 201. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 49:1  pp. 125 ff. DOI logo
Mao, Tiaoyuan
2020. Redefining pragmatic competence among modular interactions and beyond. Intercultural Pragmatics 17:5  pp. 605 ff. DOI logo
Mao, Tiaoyuan
2023. Pragmatic competence, autistic language use and the basic properties of human language. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 53:3  pp. 314 ff. DOI logo
Németh T., Enikő
2004. The principles of communicative language use. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 51:3-4  pp. 379 ff. DOI logo
Schnell, Zsuzsanna
2019. Fuzzy Boundaries in Interpretation: Empirical Study on the Common Traits and Differences of Irony and Metaphor as the Hypothesized Infringements of the Maxim of Quality. In Fuzzy Boundaries in Discourse Studies,  pp. 151 ff. DOI logo
Schnell, Zsuzsanna
2022. Preschoolers’ Sensitivity to the Infringement of Conversational Maxims in View of Mentalization. In A Life in Cognition [Language, Cognition, and Mind, 11],  pp. 137 ff. DOI logo
Schnell, Zsuzsanna, Eszter Varga, Tamás Tényi, Mária Simon, András Hajnal, Róbert Járai & Róbert Herold
2016. Neuropragmatics and irony processing in schizophrenia – Possible neural correlates of the meta-module of pragmatic meaning construction. Journal of Pragmatics 92  pp. 74 ff. DOI logo
Xu, Chang, Tiaoyuan Mao & Shengbin Du
2023. Divergent social communication between autistic and non-autistic individuals revisited: unraveled via an Integrated Model of Pragmatic Competence. Frontiers in Psychology 14 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.