Article published In:
Issues in Humour Cognition
Edited by Marta Dynel
[Review of Cognitive Linguistics 16:1] 2018
► pp. 128151
References
Alverson, H.
(1994) Semantics and experience: Universal metaphors of time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Barlow, M., & Kemmer, S.
(Eds.) (2000) Usage-based models of language. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Bybee, J.
(2006) From usage to grammar: the mind’s response to repetition. Language, 82(4), 711–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casini, L., & Macar, F.
(1997) Effects of attention manipulation on perceived duration and intensity in the visual modality. Memory and Cognition, 25(6), 812–818. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A.
(2004) Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Davison, M., & McCarthy, D.
(1988) The matching law: A research review. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Deckert, M., & Pęzik, P.
(2014) Degrees of propositionality in construals of time quantities. Research in Language, 12(4), 341–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L.
(1998) Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(8), 355–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, V.
(2004) The structure of time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fedden, S., & Boroditsky, L.
(2012) Spatialization of time in Mian. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 485.Google Scholar
Flaherty, M.
(1999) A watched pot: How we experience time. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R.
(1990) The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000) Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychological Review, 107(2), 289–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R.
(2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2), 59–65. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L.
(2002) As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space→time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(5), 537–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivry, R. B., & Hazeltine, R. E.
(1995) Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: Evidence of a common timing mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(1), 3–18.Google Scholar
Jones, L. A., Poliakoff, E., & Wells, J.
(2009) Good vibrations: Human interval timing in the vibrotactile modality. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(11), 2171–2186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W.
(1987) Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
(2000) A dynamic usage-based model. In M. Barlow & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Usage-based models of language (pp. 1–63). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
(2008) Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. New York: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leon, M. I., & Shadlen, M. N.
(2003) Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron, 38(2), 317–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthews, W. J.
(2011) How do changes in speed affect the perception of duration? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1617–1627.Google Scholar
Matthews, W. J., Stewart, N., & Wearden, J. H.
(2011) Stimulus intensity and the perception of duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(1), 303–313.Google Scholar
Núñez, R., Motz, B., & Teuscher, U.
(2006) Time after time: The psychological reality of the ego- and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(3), 133–146. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E.
(2006) With the future behind them: convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401–450. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pariyadath, V., & Eagleman, D. M.
(2007) The effect of predictability on subjective duration. PLoS One, 2(11), e1264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Brief subjective durations contract with repetition. Journal of Vision, 8(16), 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pęzik, P.
(2012) Wyszukiwarka PELCRA dla danych NKJP. In A. Przepiórkowski, M. Bańko, R. L. Górski, & B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Eds.), Narodowy Korpus Języka Polskiego (pp. 253–273). Warsaw: PWN.Google Scholar
Rao, S. M., Mayer, A. R., & Harrington, D. L.
(2001) The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nature Neuroscience, 41, 317–323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tse, P. U., Intriligator, J., Rivest, J., & Cavanagh, P.
(2004) Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 661, 1171–1189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Walsh, V.
(2003) A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xuan, B., Zhang, D., He, S., & Chen, X.
(2007) Larger stimuli are judged to last longer. Journal of Vision, 7(10), 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar