Regular articles
Constructions at work in foreign language learners’ mind
A comparison between two sentence-sorting experiments with English and Italian learners
This article reports empirical evidence of constructional priming effects in L2 learners of English and Italian.
The well-known pioneering experiment carried out by
Bencini and Goldberg (2000) with L1
speakers of English paved the way for our investigation. We employed the same protocol to ascertain whether constructions have an
ontological status also in the mind of L2 learners. We conducted experiments with four groups of learners whose language
proficiency levels correspond to the B1 and B2 levels of the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR). The
results we obtained in our cross-linguistic experiments demonstrate that learners are reliant on constructional templates when
they are required to produce linguistic generalizations.
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Priming
- 3.Priming experiment with native speakers of American English
- 4.Experiment with Italian university learners of English
- 4.1L2 pioneering studies
- 4.2Constructional priming in Italian learners of English
- 4.2.1Materials
- 4.2.2Procedure
- 4.2.3Scoring
- 4.2.4Results
- 4.3An interim observation on language typology
- 5.Constructional priming in Romance-language-speaking learners of Italian
- 5.1Stimuli
- 5.2Procedure
- 5.3Scoring
- 5.4Results
- 6.Discussion of the results
- 7.Pedagogical implications
- Acknowledgements
- Notes
-
References
References (58)
References
Achard, M. (2008). Teaching construal: Cognitive pedagogical grammar. In P. Robinson & N. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 432–455). New York: Routledge.
Baicchi, A. (2015). Construction learning as a complex adaptive system: Psycholinguistic evidence from L2 learners of English. Berlin: Springer.
Baicchi, A. (2016). The role of syntax and semantics in constructional priming: Experimental evidence from Italian university learners of English through a sentence-elicitation task. In S. de Knop & G. Gilquin (Eds.), Applied construction grammar (pp. 211–236). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bencini, G., & Goldberg, A. (2000). The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 431, 640–651.
Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 181, 355–387.
Boers, F. (2013). Cognitive Linguistic approaches to second language vocabulary: Assessment and integration. Language Teaching, 461, 208–224.
De Knop, S., Boers, F., & de Rycker, A. (Eds.). (2010). Fostering language teaching efficiency through cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
De Rycker, A., & De Knop, S. (2008) (Eds.). Cognitive approaches to pedagogical grammar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
De Rycker, A., & De Knop, S. (2009). Integrating Cognitive Linguistics and foreign language teaching: Historical background and new developments. Journal of Modern Languages, 19(1), 29–46.
Della Putta, P. (2015). Discouraging constructional negative transfer: Theoretical aspects and classroom activities for Spanish-speaking students of L2 Italian. In K. Masuda, C. Arnett, & A. Labarca (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory (pp. 25–49). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Desmet, T., & Declercq, M. (2006). Cross-linguistic priming of syntactic hierarchical configuration information. Journal of Memory and Language, 541, 610–632.
Eddington, D., & Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J. (2010). Argument structure constructions and language processing. In S. de Knop, F. Boers, & A. de Rycher (Eds.), Fostering language teaching efficiency through cognitive linguistics (pp. 213–238). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2010). The bounds of adult language acquisition: Blocking and learned attention. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(4), 553–580.
Ellis, N. C., & Sagarra, N. (2011). Learned attention in adult language acquisition: a replication and generalization study and meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 331, 589–624.
Feldman, S., & Weld, H. P. (1939). Perception. In E. G. Boring, S. H. Langfeld, H. P. Weld, et al. (Eds.), Introduction to psychology (pp. 411–438). New York: John Wiley.
Ferreira, V. (2003). The persistence of optional complementizer production. Journal of Memory and Language, 481, 379–398.
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hartsuiker, R., Kolk, H., & Hiuskamp, P. (1999). Priming word order in sentence production. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A1, 129–147.
Hartsuiker, R., Pickering, M., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Psychological Science, 151, 409–414.
Healy, A., & Miller, G. (1970). The verb as the main determinant of sentence meaning. Psychonomic Science, 201, 372.
Holme, R. (2012). Cognitive linguistics and the second language class. TESOL Quarterly, 461, 6–29.
Jacobsen, N. (2015). Using cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory for second language teaching. In K. Masuda, C. Arnett, & A. Labarca (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and sociocultural theory (pp. 103–126). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Kersten, A., & Billman, D. (1997). Event category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 231, 638–658.
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104(2), 211–240.
Landauer, T., Foltz, P., & Laham, D. (1998). An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 251, 259–284.
Lashley, K. (1951). The problem of serial order in behaviour. In L. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behaviour (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley.
Lassaline, M., & Murphy, G. (1996). Induction and category coherence. Psychonomic Bullettin and Review, 3(1), 95–99.
Liang, J. (2002). How do Chinese EFL learners construct sentence meaning. M.A. thesis, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies.
Libben, G., & Jarema, G. (2002). Mental lexicon research in the new millennium. Brain and Language, 81(1–3), 2–11.
Littlemore, J. (2009). Applying cognitive linguistics to second language learning and teaching. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Loebell, H., & Bock, K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics, 411, 791–794.
Mateu, J., & Rigau, G. (2010). Verb-particle constructions in Romance: A lexical-syntactic account. Probus, 221, 241–269.
Martínez Vázquez, M. (2004). Learning argument structure generalizations in a foreign language. VIAL, 11, 151–165.
Medin, D. L., Wattemaker, W., & Hampson, S. (1987). Family resemblance, conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction. Cognitive Psychology, 121, 242–279.
Meyer, D., & Schvaneveldt, R. (1971). Facilitation in recognising pairs of words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 901, 227–234.
Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Noora, S. (2009). Iranian undergraduate non-English majors’ interpretation of English structures. GEMA, 9(2), 89–100.
Pedersen, J. (2009b). Lexical and constructional organization of argument structure: A contrastive analysis. In J. Zlatev, M. Andrén, M. Johansson Falck, & C. Lundmark (Eds.), Studies in language and cognition (pp. 230–245). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars.
Regehr, G., & Brooks, L. R. (1995). Category organization in free classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 211, 347–363.
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J., & Mairal Usón, R. (2008). Levels of description and constraining factors in meaning construction: An introduction to the lexical constructional model. Folia Linguistica, 42(2), 355–400.
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J., & Agustín Llach, M. (2016). Cognitive Pedagogical Grammar and meaning construction in L2. In S. de Knop & G. Gilquin (Eds.), Applied construction grammar (pp. 151–184). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Segal, S. J., & Cofer, C. N. (1960). The effect of recency and recall on word association. American Psychologist, 151, 451.
Sepassi, F., & Kamyab, P. (2005). Iranian university students’ preference for verb centered vs. construction cues to sentence structure. The Asian EFL Journal, 7(5). Retrieved May 13, 2018, from [URL]
Sinha, C. & Kuteva, T. (1995). Distributed Spatial Semantics. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 181, 167–199.
Storms, L. H. (1958). Apparent backward association: a situational effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 390–395.
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Torre, E. (2012). Symmetry and asymmetry in Italian caused-motion constructions: An Embodied Construction Grammar approach. Constructions, 11. Retrieved from: [URL]
Tulving, E., Schacter, D. L., & Stark, H. A. (1982). Priming effects in word-fragment completion are independent of recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(4), 336–342.
Valenzuela, J., & Rojo, A. (2007). On the existence of constructions in foreign language learners. In R. Monroy & A. Sánchez (Eds.), 25 años de Lingüística en España (pp. 907–912). Murcia: Editum.
Washio, R. (1997). Resultatives, compositionality and language variation. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 61, 1–49.
Wolter, B., & Yamashita, J. (2018). Word frequency, collocational frequency, L1 congruency, and proficiency in L2 collocational processing: What accounts for L2 performance? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(2), 395–416.
Cited by (1)
Cited by one other publication
Park, Ji-Hyun & Min-Chang Sung
2024.
Expansion of verb-argument construction repertoires in L2 English writing.
International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 62:2
► pp. 903 ff.
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 15 july 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.