Article published in:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 17:2 (2019) ► pp. 411437
References
Andrén, M.
(2010) Children’s gestures from 18 to 30 months. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Lund University.Google Scholar
Calbris, G.
(2003) From cutting an object to a clear-cut analysis: Gesture as the representation of a preconceptual schema linking concrete actions to abstract notions. Gesture, 3 (1), 19–46. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A.
(1998a) Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In J.-P. Koenig (Ed.), Discourse and cognition: Bridging the gap (pp. 189–204). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
(1998b)  straight: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9 1, 107–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1999) Metaphors and cultural models as profiles and bases. In R. W. Gibbs, Jr., & G. J. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 189–203). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Image schemas and gesture. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 421–442). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11 (2), 417–432. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G.
(1985/1994) Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1997) Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M.
(1998) Conceptual integration networks. Cognitive Science, 22 (2), 133–187. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2002) The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C.
(2007) Environmentally coupled gestures. In S. D. Duncan, J. Cassell, & E. T. Levy (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language (pp. 195–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hampe, B., in coop. with J. Grady
(Eds.) (2005) From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B.
(2011) When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137 (2), 297–315. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E.
(1995) Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.
(1987) The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.
(2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., Alibali, M. W., & Chu, M.
(2017) How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 124 (3), 245–266. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H.
(2011) Putting the cyclic gesture on a cognitive basis. CogniTextes, 6 1, http://​cognitextes​.revues​.org​/406. Crossref
(2014) The cyclic gesture. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & S. Tessendorf (Eds.), Body – language – communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction, Vol. 2 1 (pp. 1605–1618). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1993) The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought, 2nd ed. (pp. 202–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
LeBaron, C., & Streeck, J.
(2000) Gestures, knowledge, and the world. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 118–138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, Scott K.
(1998) Grounded blends, gestures, and conceptual shifts. Cognitive Linguistics, 9 1, 283–314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M.
(1992) How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99 1, 587–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought, 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2005) How to build a baby: III. Image schemas and the transition to verbal thought. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 137–163). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Cánovas, C. P.
(2014) On defining image schemas. Language and Cognition, 6 (4), 510–532. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.
(1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2015) Why we gesture: The surprising role of hand movements in communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, I.
(2010) Geometric and image-schematic patterns in gesture space. In V. Evans & P. Chilton (Eds.), Language, cognition and space: The state of the art and new directions (pp. 351–385). London: Equinox.Google Scholar
(2018) Gestures as image schemas and force gestalts: A dynamic systems approach augmented with motion-capture analyses. Cognitive Semiotics, 11 (1), 20180002, ISSN (Online) 2235–2066. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Núñez, R.
(2007) The cognitive science of mathematics: Why is it relevant for mathematics education? In R. A. Lesh, E. Hamilton, & J. J. Kaput (Eds.), Foundations for the future in mathematics education (pp. 127–154). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Perrill, F., & Sweetser, E.
(2004) What we mean by meaning: Conceptual integration in gesture analysis and transcription. Gesture, 4 (2), 197–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C.
(2014) Living in the model: The cognitive ecology of time – a comparative study. In L. Magnani (Ed.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology (pp. 55–73). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, N.
(2007) Gesture without interaction: Cognitive uses for a communicative capacity. Paper presented at the third conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, Evanston, IL, June 18–21.
Streeck, J.
(2011) Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Talmy, L.
(2000) Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 1: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tessendorf, S.
(2014) Pragmatic and metaphoric – combining functional and cognitive approaches in the analysis of the ‘brushing aside gesture.’ In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & S. Tessendorf (Eds.), Body – language – communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction, vol. 2 1 (pp. 1540–1558). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Williams, R. F.
(2004) Making meaning from a clock: Material artifacts and conceptual blending in time-telling instruction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
(2007a) Counting and conceptual blending. Paper presented at the tenth International Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Krakow, Poland.
(2007b) Using mapping and anchoring gestures to establish common ground. Paper presented at the third conference of the International Society for Gesture Studies, Evanston, IL, June 18–21.
(2008a) Gesture as a conceptual mapping tool. In A. Cienki & C. Müller (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 55–92). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008b) Guided conceptualization: Mental spaces in instructional discourse. In T. Oakley & A. Hougaard (Eds.), Mental spaces in discourse and interaction (pp. 209–234). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Image schemas in clock-reading: Latent errors and emerging expertise. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21 (2), 216–246. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Distributed cognition and gesture. In C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. Ladewig, D. McNeill, & S. Tessendorf (Eds.), Body – language – communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction, vol. 1 1 (pp. 240–258). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J.
(2005) What’s in a schema? Bodily mimesis and the grounding of language. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics (pp. 313–342). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) Image schemas, mimetic schemas, and children’s gestures. Cognitive Semiotics, 7 (1), 3–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar