Article published In:
Review of Cognitive Linguistics
Vol. 18:1 (2020) ► pp.213243
References
Bohnemeyer, J.
(2007) Morpholexical relatedness and the argument structure of verbs of cutting and breaking. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(2), 153–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M.
(1978) The acquisition of word meaning: An investigation into some current conflicts. In N. Waterson & C. Snow (Eds.), The development of communication (pp. 263–287). New York: Wiely.Google Scholar
(2005) Why can’t you “open” a nut or “break” a cooked noodle? Learning covert object categories in action word meanings. In L. Gershkoff-Stowe & D. Raikson (Eds.), Building object categories in developmental time: 32nd carnegie symposium on cognition (pp. 33–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M., & Choir, S.
(2001) Shaping meanings from language: Universals and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475–511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M. J.
(2013) Semantic change and cognition: How the present illuminates the past and the future. In C. Howe, S. E. Blackwell & M. L. Quesada (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 15th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 1–16). MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Chen, J. D.
(2007) ‘He cut-break the rope’: Encoding and categorizing cutting and breaking events in Mandarin. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(2), 273–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E., Carpenter, K. L., & Deutsch, W.
(1995) Reference states and reversals: Undoing actions with verbs. Journal of Child Language, (2), 633–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colleman, T., & De Clerck, B.
(2011) Constructional semantics on the move: On semantic specialization in the English double object construction. Cognitive Linguistics, 22(1),183–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W.
(2006) The relevance of an evolutionary model to historical linguistics. In T. O. Nedergaard (Ed.). Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond (pp. 91–132). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The origins of grammaticalization in the verbalization of experience. Linguistics, 48(1), 1–48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012) Verbs: Aspect and causal structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Force dynamics and directed change in event lexicalization and argument realization. In R. G. de Almeida & C. Manouilidou (Eds.), Cognitive science perspectives on verb representation and processing (pp. 103–129). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A.
(2004) Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Essegbey, J.
(2007) Cut and break verbs in Sranan. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(2), 231–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Firth, J.
(1957) Papers in linguistics. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Franco, K., Geeraerts, D., Speelman, D., & Van Hout, R.
(2019) Concept characteristics and variation in lexical diversity in two Dutch dialect areas. Cognitive Linguistics, 30(1), 205–242. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fujii, S., Radetzky, P., & Sweetser, E.
(2013) Splitting, cutting and breaking talk in Japanese. Paper presented at the 12th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Edmonton: University of Alberta.
Geeraerts, D.
(1997) Diachronic prototype semantics: A contribution to historical lexicology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
(2000) Salience phenomena in the lexicon: A typology. In L. Albertazzi (Ed.), Meaning and cognition (pp. 79–101). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Theories of lexical semantics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D., Kristiansen, G., & Peirsman, Y.
(2010) Advances in Cognitive Sociolinguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D.
(2018) Ten lectures on Cognitive Sociolinguistics. Leiden: Brill.Google Scholar
Glynn, D.
(2014) Correspondence analysis: Exploring data and identifying patterns. In D. Glynn & J. A. Robinson (Eds.), Corpus methods for semantics: Quantitative studies in polysemy and synonymy (pp. 443–486). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenacre, M. J.
(2007) Correspondence analysis in practice (2nd ed). Barcelona: Universitat Pompeu Fabra. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guerssel, M., Hale, K., Laughren, M., Levin, B., & Eagle, J. W.
(1985) A crosslinguistic study of transitivity alternations. In W. H. Eilfort, P. D. Kroeber & K. L. Peterson (Eds.), Papers from the parasession on causatives and agentivity at the 21st Regional Meeting (pp. 48–63). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Györi, G.
(1996) Historical aspects of categorization. In E. H. Casad (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics in the redwoods: The expansion of a new paradigm in linguistics (pp. 175–206). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hu, C.
(2005) The early forms of resultative construction and relevant criterion. Chinese Language, (3), 214–225.Google Scholar
(胡敕瑞,
2005,动结式的早期形式及判定标准 ,《中国语文》(3):214–225)Google Scholar
Hull, D. L.
(1988) Science as a process: An evolutionary account of the social and conceptual development of science. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G.
(1987) Cognitive models and prototype theory. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Emory symposia in cognition, 1. Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 63–100). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R.
(2008) Cognitive Grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levin, B.
(1993) English verb classes and alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M.
(1995) Unaccusativity: At the syntax-lexical semantics interface. Massachusetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levshina, N.
Majid, A., Bowerman, M., van Staden, M., & Boster, J. S.
(2007) The semantic categories of breaking and cutting events: A crosslinguistic perspective. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(2), 133–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Rosch, E.
(1981) Categorization of natural objects. Annual Review of Psychology, (32), 89–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pye, C.
(1996) K’iche’ Maya verbs of breaking and cutting. In M. Goodel & D. I. Choi (Eds.), Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics 211 (pp. 87–98). Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas.Google Scholar
Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B.
(2001) An event structure account of English resultatives. Language, 77, 766–797.Google Scholar
(2005) Change of state verbs: Implications for theories of argument projection. In N. Erteschik-Shir & R. Tova (Eds.), The syntax of aspect: Deriving thematic and aspectual interpretation (pp. 274–287). Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E.
(1973) On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). New York: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1975) Cognitive representation of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 104(3), 192–233. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1978) Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B.
(1975) Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, (7), 573–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shaefer, R. P.
(1980) An experimental assessment of the boundaries demarcating three basic semantic categories in the domain of separation. University of Kansas, Unpublished doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Soares da Silva, A.
(2003) Image schemas and category coherence: The case of the Portuguese verb deixar. In H. Cuyckens, R. Dirven & J. R. Taylor (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to lexical semantics (pp. 281–322). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Verbs of letting: Some cognitive and historical aspects. In N. Delbecque & B. Cornille (Eds.), On interpreting construction schemas: From action and motion to transitivity and causality (pp. 171–200). Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Talmy, L.
(2000a) Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. I: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2000b) Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. II: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. R.
(2002) Near synonyms as co-extensive categories: ‘high’ and ‘tall’ revisited. Language Sciences, 251, 263–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2003) Linguistic categorization (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Traugott, E., & Dasher, R. B.
(2002) Regularity in Semantic Change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wang, L.
(2013/1957–1958) Outline of the History of Chinese. Beijing: Zhong Hua Book Company.Google Scholar
(王力
2013(1957–1958)《汉语史稿》,北京:中华书局。)Google Scholar
Wedel, A. B.
(2006) Exemplar models, evolution and language change. The Linguistic Review, 231, 247–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winters, M. E.
(1987) Syntactic and semantic space: The development of the English subjective. In A. Giacalone- Ramat, C. Onofrio & B. Giuliano (Eds.), Papers from the 7th International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Berlin & New York: John Benjamins. DOI logo
(2010) Introduction: on the emergence of diachronic cognitive linguistics. In M. E. Winters, H. Tissari & K. Allan (Eds.), Historical Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 3–31). New York & Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xu, D.
(2001) A study on the semantic influence on syntactic structure from the formation of resultative constructions – Division of semantic and function of Chinese verbs. Linguistic Researches, (21), 5–12.Google Scholar
(徐丹,
2001,从动补结构的形成看语义对句法结构的影响—兼谈汉语动词语义功能的分化,《语文研究》(2):5–12。)Google Scholar
(2006) Typological Change in Chinese Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar