Article published In:
The Acquisition and Processing of Spanish and Portuguese Morphosyntax: Theoretical and experimental issues
Edited by Rachel Klassen, Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Joanne Markle LaMontagne and Almudena Basanta y Romero-Valdespino
[Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics 29:2] 2016
► pp. 350395
References
Aguado-Orea, J., & Pine, J
(2002) There is no evidence for a ‘no overt subject’ stage in early child Spanish: A note on Grinstead (2000). Journal of Child Language, 291, 865–874. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Austin, J., Blume, M., Parkinson, D., Lust, B.C., & Núñez del Prado, Z
(1997) The status of pro-crop in the initial state. In A.T. Pérez-Leroux & W. Glass (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the acquisition of Spanish (pp. 37–54). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Barbosa, P
(2009) Two kinds of subject pro . Studia Linguistica, 631, 2–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bates, E
(1976) Language and context. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bel, A
(2001) Sujetos nulos y sujetos explícitos en las gramáticas iniciales del castellano y el catalán. Revista Española de Lingüística, 31(2), 537–562.Google Scholar
(2003) The syntax of subjects in the acquisition of Spanish and Catalan. Probus: An International Journal of Latin and Romance Linguistics, 15(1), 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P
(1990) Subjectless sentences in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 21(4), 491–504.Google Scholar
Brown, R
(1973) A first language: The early stages. London: George Allen and Unwin Ltd. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burzio, L
(1986) Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Camacho, J
(2013) Null subjects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N
(1981) Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A
(1990) The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. New York/Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Quay, S
(2000) Bilingual acquisition: Theoretical implications of a case study. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gavarró, A., & Cabré-Sans, Y
(2009) Subjects, verb classes and word order in child Catalan. In J. Grinstead (Ed.), Perspectives on typical and atypical Hispanic language development (pp. 175–194). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Genesee, F
(1989) Early bilingual development: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 16(1), 161–179. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grinstead, J
(1998) Subjects, sentential negation and imperatives in child Spanish and Catalan. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
(2000) Tense, number and nominative case assignment in child Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 119–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Subjects and interface delay in child Spanish and Catalan. Language, 80(1), 40–72. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grinstead, J., & Spinner, P
(2009) The clausal left periphery in child Spanish and German. Probus: An International Journal of Latin and Romance Linguistics, 211, 51–82. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V.F., Simon-Cereijido, G., & Wagner, C
(2008) Bilingual children with language impairment: A comparison with monolinguals and second language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 291, 2–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hacohen, A., & Schaeffer, J
(2007) Subject realization in early Hebrew/English bilingual acquisition: The role of crosslinguistic influence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(3), 333–344. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B
(2010) Transfer at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Pronominal subjects in bilingual Turkish. Second Language Research, 26(3), 355–378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N
(2000) Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 227–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N
(1986) Language acquisition and the theory of parameters. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyams, N., & Wexler, K
(1993) On the grammatical basis of null subjects in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 24(3), 421–459.Google Scholar
Jaeggli, O
(1984) Subject extraction and the null subject parameter. NELS, 141, 132–153.Google Scholar
Juan-Garau, M., & Pérez-Vidal, C
(2000) Subject realization in the syntactic development of a bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3(3), 173–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, M
(1989) The null subject parameter in Modern Arabic dialects. In O. Jaeggli & K. Safir (Eds.), The null subject parameterThe null subject parameter (pp. 263–276). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T
(2007) Determiners in bilingual German-Italian children: What they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 101, 57–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J.M., Fernández Fuertes, R., & Alba de la Fuente, A
(2012) Subject and copula omission in the English grammar of English-Spanish bilinguals: On the issue of directionality of interlinguistic influence. First Language, 321, 88–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J.M., Fernández Fuertes, R., & Pérez-Tattam, R
(2008) Null and overt subjects in the developing grammars (L1 English/L1 Spanish) of two bilingual twins. In A.B. Gaya (Ed.), A portrait of the young in the new multilingual Spain (pp. 111–134). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Liceras, J., Fernández Fuertes, R., Perales, S., Pérez-Tattam, R., & Todd Spradlin, K
(2008) Gender and gender agreement in bilingual native and non-native grammars: A view from child and adult functional-lexical mixings. Lingua, 118(6), 827–851. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Linaza, J., Sebastián, M.E., & del Barrio, C
(1981) Lenguaje, comunicación y comprensión. La adquisición del lenguaje. Special issue of Infancia y Aprendizaje, 195–198.Google Scholar
Lorusso, P
(2014) The acquisition of the primitive elements of the VP at the syntax-semantics interface. Doctoral dissertation. Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B
(2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. 3rd Ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Meisel, J.M
(1989) Early differentiation of language in bilingual children. In K. Hyltenstam & L. Obler (Eds.), Bilingualism across a lifespan: Aspects of acquisition, maturity and loss. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N., & Hulk, A
(2001) Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual language acquisition: Italian and French as recipient languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4(1), 1–21. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ojea, A.I
(1997) Categorías funcionales y adquisición de la primera lengua: Un análisis contrastivo. Revista Española de Lingüística, 271, 425–446.Google Scholar
Olarrea, A
(2012) Word order and information structure. In J.I. Hualde, A. Olarrea, & E. O’Rourke (Eds.), The handbook of Hispanic linguistics (pp. 603–628). Cambridge, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ortega-Santos, I
(2008) Projecting subjects in Spanish and English. Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Otheguy, R., & Zentella, A.C
(2012) Spanish in New York: Language contact, dialectal leveling and structural continuity. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Otheguy, R., Zentella, A.C., & Livert, D
(2007) Language and dialect contact in Spanish in New York: Towards the formation of a speech community. Language, 83(4), 770–802. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Navarro, S
(2003) Subject realization and crosslinguistic interference in the bilingual acquisition of Spanish and English: What is the role of the input? Journal of Child Language, 30(2), 371–393. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Bazán, M.J
(2002) Predicting early bilingual development: Towards a probabilistic model of analysis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. UMI: Ann Harbor, Michigan.Google Scholar
Pierce, A
(1989) On the emergence of syntax: A crosslinguistic study. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
(1992) Language acquisition and syntactic theory: A comparative analysis of French and English child grammars, 14(14), Dordrecht/Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rizzi, L
(1982) Issues in Italian syntax. Dordrecht: Foris. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Early null subjects and root null subjects. In B. Lust, G. Hermon, & J. Kornfilt (Eds.), Binding, dependencies and learnability, 21 (pp. 249–272). Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schmitz, K., Patuto, M., & Müller, N
(2012) The null-subject parameter at the interface between syntax and pragmatics: Evidence from bilingual German-Italian, German-French and Italian-French children. First Language, 32(1-2), 205–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., & Hervé, C
(2015) Referential expressions in bilingual acquisition. In L. Serratrice & S.E.M. Allen (Eds.), The acquisition of reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S
(2004) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subject and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(3), 183–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., Filiaci, F., & Baldo, M
(2009) Bilingual children’s sensitivity to specificity and genericity: Evidence from metalinguistic awareness. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(2), 239–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C
(2012) Acquisition of Spanish in bilingual contexts. In J.I. Hualde, A. Olarrea, & E. O’Rourke (Eds.), The handbook of Hispanic linguistics (pp. 783–802). Cambridge, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Bilingual language acquisition: Spanish and English in the first six years. New York: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W., & Stromswold, K
(1997) The structure and acquisition of English dative constructions. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 281–317.Google Scholar
Snyder, W
(2007) Child language: The parametric approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Soares, C
(2003) The C-domain and the acquisition of European Portuguese. Probus, 15(1), 147–176. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A
(2004) Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax-discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 143–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, K
(1996) Analyzing children’s spontaneous speech. In D. McDaniel, C. McKee, & H. Smith-Cairns (Eds.), Methods for assessing children’s syntax (pp. 23–53). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Valian, V
(1991) Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 401, 21–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vila, I
(1990) Adquisición y desarrollo del lenguaje. Barcelona: Graó.Google Scholar
Villa-García, J
(2009) Overt subjects, wh-questions and fronted constituents in child Spanish: The view from the time-course of acquisition, dialectal microvariation and grammatical conservatism. Unpublished manuscript. University of Connecticut, Storrs.Google Scholar
(2011) Acquisitional evidence bearing on the account of preverbal and postverbal subjects in Spanish. In L. Ortiz (Ed.), Selected proceedings of the 13th Hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 179–189). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2012) Spanish subjects can be subjects: Acquisitional and empirical evidence. Iberia: An International Journal of Theoretical Linguistics, 4(2), 124–169.Google Scholar
(2013) On the role of children’s deterministic learning in the ‘no-overt-subject’ stage in the L1 acquisition of Spanish. In C. Cathcart, I.H. Chen, G Finley, S. Kang, C.S. Sandy, & E. Stickles (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 375–388). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Villa-García, J., & Snyder, W
(2010) On the acquisition of overt subjects, topics, and wh-questions in Spanish. In A. Castro, J. Costa, M. Lobo, & F. Pratas (Eds.), Language acquisition and development: Proceedings of GALA 2009 (pp. 493–504). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., & Taeschner, T
(1978) The acquisition and development of language by bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5(2), 311–326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S
(2009) Cross-linguistic influence in bilingual and multilingual contexts. Invited paper at the 2009 international symposium on bilingualism (ISB7) . University of Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Zwanziger, E.E., Allen, S.E., & Genesee, F
(2005) Crosslinguistic influence in bilingual acquisition: Subject omission in learners of Inuktitut and English. Journal of Child Language, 32(4), 893–909. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Frasson, Alberto
2021. Clitics are not enough: on agreement and null subjects in Brazilian Venetan. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 6:1 DOI logo
Osch, Brechje van & Petra Sleeman
Sánchez Calderón, Silvia
2021. Simple Monotransitive and Complex Dative Alternation Predicates in Spanish Monolingual Child Acquisition Data. Probus 33:1  pp. 151 ff. DOI logo
Sánchez Calderón, Silvia
2021. Simple Monotransitive and Complex Dative Alternation Predicates in Spanish Monolingual Child Acquisition Data. Probus 33:1  pp. 151 ff. DOI logo
van Osch, García González, Hulk, Sleeman & Aalberse
2019. The Development of Subject Position in Dutch-Dominant Heritage Speakers of Spanish: From Age 9 to Adulthood. Languages 4:4  pp. 88 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.