Article published in:
The Acquisition and Processing of Spanish and Portuguese Morphosyntax: Theoretical and experimental issues
Edited by Rachel Klassen, Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Joanne Markle LaMontagne and Almudena Basanta y Romero-Valdespino
[Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics 29:2] 2016
► pp. 487522
References

References

Arregui, A.
(2007) When aspect matters: the case of would-conditionals. Natural Language Semantics, 15(3), 221–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R.C., & Weinberg, A.S.
(1984) The grammatical basis of linguistic performance: Language use and acquisition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D.L.
(1956) Subjunctive -ra and -se: “Free variation”? Hispania, 39, 345–349. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J., & Prévost, P.
(2008) Methodological issues in the L2 acquisition of a syntax/semantics phenomenon: How to assess L2 knowledge of mood in Spanish relative clauses. In J. Bruhn de Garavito & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 10th Hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 13–24). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
(2015) Mood selection in relative clauses. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1), 33–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Collentine, J.
(1995) The development of complex syntax and mood-selection abilities by intermediate-level learners of Spanish. Hispania, 78(1), 122–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1998) Processing instruction and the subjunctive. Hispania, 81, 576–587. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) The acquisition and teaching of the Spanish subjunctive: An update on current findings. Hispania, 93(1), 39–51.Google Scholar
Crutchley, A.
(2004) ‘If she had of shutted the cage, the rabbit wouldn’t escape’: Past counterfactuals elicited from 6-to 11-year-old children. First Language, 24(2), 209–240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Structure of child and adult past counterfactuals, and implications for acquisition of the construction. Journal of Child Language, 40(2), 438–468. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J.
(2007) The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua, 117(11), 1858–1878. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S.D., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G.
(1996) Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 677–758. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Eubank, L., Bischof, J., Huffstutler, A., Leek, P., & West, C.
(1997) “Tom eats slowly cooked eggs”: Thematic-verb raising in L2 knowledge. Language Acquisition, 6(3), 171–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Geeslin, K.L., & Gudmestad, A.
(2010) An exploration of the range and frequency of occurrence of forms in potentially variable structures in second-language Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(3), 433–463. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., & Bever, T.G.
(1986) Linguistic intuitions are the result of interactions between perceptual processes and linguistic universals. Cognitive Science, 10(4), 457–476. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, N.
(1983) Fact, fiction, and forecast. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grondin, N., & White, L.
(1996) Functional categories in child L2 acquisition of French. Language Acquisition, 5(1), 1–34. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gudmestad, A.
(2006) L2 variation and the Spanish subjunctive: Linguistic features predicting mood selection. In C.A. Klee & T.L. Face (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 7th conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as first and second languages (pp. 170–184). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
(2008) Acquiring a variable structure: An interlanguage analysis of second-language mood use in Spanish. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 1(2), 449–449. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Moving beyond a sentence-level analysis in the study of variable mood use in Spanish. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 29(1), 25–52.Google Scholar
(2012) On the role of lexical items in the second-language development of mood use in Spanish. In R.T. Miller, K.I. Martin, C.M. Eddington, A. Henery, N.M. Miguel, A. Tseng, A. Tuninetti, & D. Walter (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the second language research forum (pp. 120–133). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
(2013) Tense-aspect distinctions within the subjunctive mood in the Spanish of native speakers and second-language learners. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 6(1), 3–36. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C.Y.H.
(1997) The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional features hypothesis’. Second Language Research, 13(3), 187–226. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B.
(2001) The acquisition of the IP system in child L2 English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(1), 1–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B., & Schwartz, B.D.
(1997) Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 257–268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Hoeck, N.V., Begtas, E., Steen, J., Kestemont, J., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F.
(2014) False belief and counterfactual reasoning in a social environment. Neuroimage, 90, 315–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2004) Syntactic and interface knowledge in advanced and near-native interlanguage grammars. EUROSLA Yearbook, 4(1), 67–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iatridou, S.
(2000) The grammatical ingredients of counterfactuality. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 231–270. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, M., Kempchinsky, P., & Rothman, J.
(2008) Interface vulnerability and knowledge of the subjunctive/indicative distinction with negated epistemic predicates in L2 Spanish. EUROSLA Yearbook, 8(1), 135–163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T, & Wexler, K.
(2002) Why is ‘is’ easier than ‘-s’?: Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research, 18(2), 95–136. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ippolito, M.
(2003) Presuppositions and implicatures in counterfactuals. Natural Language Semantics, 11(2), 145–186. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laca, B.
(2009) Mood in Spanish. In B. Rothstein & R. Thieroff (Eds.), Mood in the languages of Europe (pp. 198‐220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D.
(1998) Case and tense in the ‘fossilized’ steady state. Second Language Research, 14(1), 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2000) Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Second language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 102–129). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2005) On morphological competence. In L. Dekydtspotter, R.A. Sprouse, & A. Liljestrand (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (GASLA 2004) (pp. 178–192). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Mazzoleni, M.
(1994) La semantica della scelta modale nei condizionali italiani. Revue Romane, 29(1), 17–32.Google Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2009) Knowledge of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 239–269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Multiple interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua, 121(4), 591–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Perpiñán, S.
(2011) Assessing differences and similarities between instructed heritage language learners and L2 learners in their knowledge of Spanish tense-aspect and mood (TAM) morphology. Heritage Language Journal, 8(1), 90–133.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Slabakova, R.
(2002) The L2 acquisition of morphosyntactic and semantic properties of the aspectual tenses preterite and imperfect. In A.T. Perez-Leroux & J. Liceras (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax (pp. 115–151). Netherlands: Kluwer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ogihara, T.
(2000) Counterfactuals, temporal adverbs, and association with focus. In B. Jackson & T. Matthews (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th semantics and linguistic theory conference (SALT X) (pp. 115–131). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, T.
(1998) The acquisition of mood selection in Spanish relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 25(3), 585–604. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L.
(2000) Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second language Research, 16(2), 103–133. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J.S.
(1982) The acquisition of conditionals in English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Roese, N.J.
(1997) Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133–148. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salaberry, R.
(1999) The development of past tense verbal morphology in classroom L2 Spanish. Applied Linguistics, 20(2), 151–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sánchez-Naranjo, J.
(2009) L2 learners’ difficulties in the interpretation of the Spanish subjunctive: L1 influence and misanalysis of the input. Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
(2014) Interpretation and grammar interaction in the Spanish subjunctive adjuncts. Borealis: An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 3(1), 125–154. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R.
(2003) Semantic evidence for functional categories in interlanguage grammars. Second Language Research, 19(1), 42–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006a) Is there a critical period for semantics? Second Language Research, 22(3), 302–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Learnability in the second language acquisition of semantics: A bidirectional study of a semantic parameter. Second Language Research, 22(4), 498–523. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008) Meaning in the second language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) What is easy and what is hard to acquire in a second language? In H. Caunt-Nulton, M. Bowles, I. Ionin, S. Montrul, & A. Tremblay (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (GASLA 2009) (pp. 280–294). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
(2010) Semantic theory and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 30, 231–247. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2014) The bottleneck of second language acquisition. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 46(4), 543–559.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R., & Montrul, S.
(2002) On viewpoint aspect interpretation and its L2 acquisition: A UG perspective. In R. Salaberry & Y. Shirai (Eds.), The L2 acquisition of tense-aspect morphology (pp. 363–395). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2004) Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax-discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 143–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K.P. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(1), 1–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Filiaci, F.
(2006) Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research, 22(3), 339–368. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Serratrice, L.
(2009) Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 195–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T., & Hooper, J.
(1974) A semantically based analysis of mood in Spanish. Hispania, 57(3), 484–494. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I., & Sorace, A.
(2006) Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax–seman- tics and syntax–discourse phenomena. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 653–664). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M.
(1996) Gradual development of L2 phrase structure. Second Language Research 12(1), 7–39. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B.
(2010) Some verbs are more perfect than others: Why learners have difficulty with ser and estar and what it means for instruction. Hispania, 93(1), 29–38.Google Scholar
Wellman, H.M.
(1990) The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
White, L.
(2009) Grammatical theory: Interfaces and L2 knowledge. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 49–68). Leeds, UK: Emerald.Google Scholar