Article published In:
The Acquisition and Processing of Spanish and Portuguese Morphosyntax: Theoretical and experimental issues
Edited by Rachel Klassen, Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Joanne Markle LaMontagne and Almudena Basanta y Romero-Valdespino
[Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics 29:2] 2016
► pp. 487522
References
Arregui, A
(2007) When aspect matters: the case of would-conditionals. Natural Language Semantics, 15(3), 221–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berwick, R.C., & Weinberg, A.S
(1984) The grammatical basis of linguistic performance: Language use and acquisition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D.L
(1956) Subjunctive -ra and -se: “Free variation”? Hispania, 391, 345–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borgonovo, C., Bruhn de Garavito, J., & Prévost, P
(2008) Methodological issues in the L2 acquisition of a syntax/semantics phenomenon: How to assess L2 knowledge of mood in Spanish relative clauses. In J. Bruhn de Garavito & E. Valenzuela (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 10th Hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 13–24). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
(2015) Mood selection in relative clauses. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1), 33–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collentine, J
(1995) The development of complex syntax and mood-selection abilities by intermediate-level learners of Spanish. Hispania, 78(1), 122–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Processing instruction and the subjunctive. Hispania, 811, 576–587. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) The acquisition and teaching of the Spanish subjunctive: An update on current findings. Hispania, 93(1), 39–51.Google Scholar
Crutchley, A
(2004) ‘If she had of shutted the cage, the rabbit wouldn’t escape’: Past counterfactuals elicited from 6-to 11-year-old children. First Language, 24(2), 209–240. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Structure of child and adult past counterfactuals, and implications for acquisition of the construction. Journal of Child Language, 40(2), 438–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Villiers, J
(2007) The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua, 117(11), 1858–1878. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S.D., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G
(1996) Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(4), 677–758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eubank, L., Bischof, J., Huffstutler, A., Leek, P., & West, C
(1997) “Tom eats slowly cooked eggs”: Thematic-verb raising in L2 knowledge. Language Acquisition, 6(3), 171–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geeslin, K.L., & Gudmestad, A
(2010) An exploration of the range and frequency of occurrence of forms in potentially variable structures in second-language Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(3), 433–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L., & Bever, T.G
(1986) Linguistic intuitions are the result of interactions between perceptual processes and linguistic universals. Cognitive Science, 10(4), 457–476. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goodman, N
(1983) Fact, fiction, and forecast. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grondin, N., & White, L
(1996) Functional categories in child L2 acquisition of French. Language Acquisition, 5(1), 1–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gudmestad, A
(2006) L2 variation and the Spanish subjunctive: Linguistic features predicting mood selection. In C.A. Klee & T.L. Face (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 7th conference on the acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as first and second languages (pp. 170–184). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
(2008) Acquiring a variable structure: An interlanguage analysis of second-language mood use in Spanish. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 1(2), 449–449. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Moving beyond a sentence-level analysis in the study of variable mood use in Spanish. Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 29(1), 25–52.Google Scholar
(2012) On the role of lexical items in the second-language development of mood use in Spanish. In R.T. Miller, K.I. Martin, C.M. Eddington, A. Henery, N.M. Miguel, A. Tseng, A. Tuninetti, & D. Walter (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the second language research forum (pp. 120–133). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
(2013) Tense-aspect distinctions within the subjunctive mood in the Spanish of native speakers and second-language learners. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 6(1), 3–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Chan, C.Y.H
(1997) The partial availability of Universal Grammar in second language acquisition: The ‘failed functional features hypothesis’. Second Language Research, 13(3), 187–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B
(2001) The acquisition of the IP system in child L2 English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(1), 1–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Haznedar, B., & Schwartz, B.D
(1997) Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 257–268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Hoeck, N.V., Begtas, E., Steen, J., Kestemont, J., Vandekerckhove, M., & Van Overwalle, F
(2014) False belief and counterfactual reasoning in a social environment. Neuroimage, 901, 315–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iatridou, S
(2000) The grammatical ingredients of counterfactuality. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 231–270. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iverson, M., Kempchinsky, P., & Rothman, J
Ionin, T, & Wexler, K
(2002) Why is ‘is’ easier than ‘-s’?: Acquisition of tense/agreement morphology by child second language learners of English. Second Language Research, 18(2), 95–136. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ippolito, M
(2003) Presuppositions and implicatures in counterfactuals. Natural Language Semantics, 11(2), 145–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Laca, B
(2009) Mood in Spanish. In B. Rothstein & R. Thieroff (Eds.), Mood in the languages of Europe (pp. 198‐220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D
(1998) Case and tense in the ‘fossilized’ steady state. Second Language Research, 14(1), 1–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) Mapping features to forms in second language acquisition. In J. Archibald (Ed.), Second language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 102–129). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
(2005) On morphological competence. In L. Dekydtspotter, R.A. Sprouse, & A. Liljestrand (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (GASLA 2004) (pp. 178–192). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Mazzoleni, M
(1994) La semantica della scelta modale nei condizionali italiani. Revue Romane, 29(1), 17–32.Google Scholar
Montrul, S
(2009) Knowledge of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 239–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Multiple interfaces and incomplete acquisition. Lingua, 121(4), 591–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Perpiñán, S
(2011) Assessing differences and similarities between instructed heritage language learners and L2 learners in their knowledge of Spanish tense-aspect and mood (TAM) morphology. Heritage Language Journal, 8(1), 90–133.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Slabakova, R
(2002) The L2 acquisition of morphosyntactic and semantic properties of the aspectual tenses preterite and imperfect. In A.T. Perez-Leroux & J. Liceras (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax (pp. 115–151). Netherlands: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ogihara, T
(2000) Counterfactuals, temporal adverbs, and association with focus. In B. Jackson & T. Matthews (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th semantics and linguistic theory conference (SALT X) (pp. 115–131). Ithaca, NY: Cornell University.Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, T
(1998) The acquisition of mood selection in Spanish relative clauses. Journal of Child Language, 25(3), 585–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L
(2000) Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second language Research, 16(2), 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reilly, J.S
(1982) The acquisition of conditionals in English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Roese, N.J
(1997) Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salaberry, R
(1999) The development of past tense verbal morphology in classroom L2 Spanish. Applied Linguistics, 20(2), 151–178. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sánchez-Naranjo, J
(2009) L2 learners’ difficulties in the interpretation of the Spanish subjunctive: L1 influence and misanalysis of the input. Doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
(2014) Interpretation and grammar interaction in the Spanish subjunctive adjuncts. Borealis: An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 3(1), 125–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, R
(2003) Semantic evidence for functional categories in interlanguage grammars. Second Language Research, 19(1), 42–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006a) Is there a critical period for semantics? Second Language Research, 22(3), 302–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Learnability in the second language acquisition of semantics: A bidirectional study of a semantic parameter. Second Language Research, 22(4), 498–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Meaning in the second language. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) What is easy and what is hard to acquire in a second language? In H. Caunt-Nulton, M. Bowles, I. Ionin, S. Montrul, & A. Tremblay (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th generative approaches to second language acquisition conference (GASLA 2009) (pp. 280–294). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
(2010) Semantic theory and second language acquisition. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 301, 231–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) The bottleneck of second language acquisition. Foreign Language Teaching and Research, 46(4), 543–559.Google Scholar
Slabakova, R., & Montrul, S
(2002) On viewpoint aspect interpretation and its L2 acquisition: A UG perspective. In R. Salaberry & Y. Shirai (Eds.), The L2 acquisition of tense-aspect morphology (pp. 363–395). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A
(2004) Native language attrition and developmental instability at the syntax-discourse interface: Data, interpretations and methods. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 143–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K.P. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: Reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(1), 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Filiaci, F
(2006) Anaphora resolution in near-native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research, 22(3), 339–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A., & Serratrice, L
(2009) Internal and external interfaces in bilingual language development: Beyond structural overlap. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(2), 195–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terrell, T., & Hooper, J
(1974) A semantically based analysis of mood in Spanish. Hispania, 57(3), 484–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I., & Sorace, A
(2006) Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax–seman- tics and syntax–discourse phenomena. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zaller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 653–664). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M
(1996) Gradual development of L2 phrase structure. Second Language Research 12(1), 7–39. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
VanPatten, B
(2010) Some verbs are more perfect than others: Why learners have difficulty with ser and estar and what it means for instruction. Hispania, 93(1), 29–38.Google Scholar
Wellman, H.M
(1990) The child’s theory of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
White, L
(2009) Grammatical theory: Interfaces and L2 knowledge. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 49–68). Leeds, UK: Emerald.Google Scholar