Chapter published in:
Experimental Arabic Linguistics
Edited by Dimitrios Ntelitheos and Tommi Tsz-Cheung Leung
[Studies in Arabic Linguistics 10] 2021
► pp. 109122
References
al Mannai, H., & Everatt, J.
(2005) Phonological processing skills as predictors of literacy amongst Arabic speaking Bahraini children. Dyslexia, 11, 269–291. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Alamri, A.
(2017) Phonological, semantic and root activation in spoken word recognition in Arabic: Evidence from eye movements. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Ottawa, Canada.Google Scholar
Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M.
(2004) Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm’. Cognition, 93, 79–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y.
(1999) Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73, 247–264. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Asaad, H., & Eviatar, Z.
(2014) Learning to read in Arabic: The long and winding road. Reading and Writing, 27(4), 649–664. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boudelaa, S., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(2001) Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon. Cognition, 81(1), 65–92. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brouwer, S., & Bradlow, A. R.
(2016) The temporal dynamics of spoken word recognition in adverse listening conditions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45(5), 1151–1160. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bylinskii, Z., Borkin, M. A., Kim, N. W., Pfister, H., & Oliva, A.
(2017) Eye fixation metrics for large scale evaluation and comparison of information visualizations. In M. Burch, L. Chuang, B. Fisher, A. Schmidt, & D. Weiskopf (Eds.), ETVIS: Workshop on Eye Tracking and Visualization. Mathematics and Visualization. (pp. 235–255). Chicago: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conklin, K., & Pellicer-Sánchez, A.
(2016) Using eye-tracking in applied linguistics and second language research. Second Language Research, 32(3), 453–467. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. M.
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6(1), 84–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Desroches, A. S., Joanisse, M. F., & Robertson, E. K.
(2006) Specific phonological impairments in dyslexia revealed by eyetracking. Cognition, 100, 32–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, F., Apel, J., & Henderson, J. M.
(2008) Taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 405–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U.
(1999) Causal connections in beginning reading: The importance of rhyme. Journal of Research in Reading, 22(3), 217–240. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U., & Bryant, P.
(1990) Phonological skills and learning to read. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hoffiz, B. T., III
(1995) Morphology of United Arab Emirates Arabic, Dubai dialect. Ph.D. Dissertation, The University of Arizona. http://​hdl​.handle​.net​/10150​/187179
Holm, A., Farrier, F., & Dodd, B.
(2008) Phonological awareness, reading accuracy and spelling ability of children with inconsistent phonological disorder. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43(3), 300–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A. S.
(2011) Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 151–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Idrissi, A., Prunet, J.-F., & Béland, R.
(2008) On the mental representation of Arabic roots. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(2), 221–259. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1976a) Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognitive Psychology, 8(4), 441–480. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1976b) The role of eye-fixation research in cognitive psychology. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 8(2), 139–143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kirtley, C., Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L.
(1989) Rhyme, rime and the onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 224–245. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Knoeferle, P., & Crocker, M. W.
(2007) The influence of recent scene events on spoken comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 519–543. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lahrache, S., El Ouazzani, R., & El Qadi, A.
(2018) Visualizations memorability through visual attention and image features. Procedia Computer Science, 127, 328–335. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landerl, K., Freudenthaler, H. H., Heene, M., De Jong, P. F., Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Parrila, R., & Georgiou, G. K.
(2019) Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 23(3), 220–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marquis, A.
(2016–2018) Development of an assessment tool for oral skills in Emirati Arabic speaking children. Al Ain, United Arab Emirates: United Arab Emirates University.Google Scholar
(2021) LATFA: An assessment tool for Emirati Arabic-speaking children. In D. Ntelitheos and T. Leung (Eds.), Experimental Arabic Linguistics (pp. 125–142). John Benjamins Publishing Company. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(1987) Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1–2), 71–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L.
(1986) The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C.
(2012) Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322–52. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, D.
(1994) Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52(3), 189–234. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ntelitheos, D., & Idrissi, A.
(2017) Language growth in child Emirati Arabic. In H. Ouali (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XXIX: Papers from the Annual Symposium on Arabic Linguistics, Milwaukee, Wisconsin, 2015 (pp. 229–248). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Orquin, J. L., & Loose, S. M.
(2013) Attention and choice: A review on eye movements in decision making. Acta Psychologica, 144(1), 190–206. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., Frisson, S., McElree, B., & Traxler, M. J.
(2004) Eye movements and semantic composition. In M. Carreiras and C. Clifton Jr. (Eds.), The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, ERP, and beyond (pp. 33–50). New York: Psychology Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pyykkönen-Klauck, P., & Crocker, M. W.
(2016) Attention and eye movement metrics in visual world eye tracking. In P. Knoeferle, P. Pyykkönen-Klauck, & M. W. Crocker (Eds.), Visually situated language comprehension (pp. 67–82). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Qafisheh, H. A.
(1977) A short reference grammar of Gulf Arabic. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.Google Scholar
Ratcliffe, R. R.
(2013) Morphology. In J. Owens (ed.), The Oxford handbook of Arabic linguistics (pp. 71–91). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K.
(2009) The 35th Sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(8), 1457–1506. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., Altmann, G. T., Spivey, M. J., & Hoover, M. A.
(2009) Much ado about eye movements to nothing: A response to Ferreira et al.: taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(6), 235–236. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, D. C., & Spivey, M. J.
(2000) Representation, space and Hollywood Squares: Looking at things that aren’t there anymore. Cognition, 76(3), 269–295. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rvachew, S., Royle, P., Gonnerman, L. M., Stanké, B., Marquis, A., & Herbay, A.
(2017) Development of a tool to screen risk of literacy delays in French-speaking children: PHOPHLO. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 41, 321–340.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E.
(2008) On the challenges posed by diglossia to the acquisition of basic reading processes in Arabic. Language and Literacy, 1, 101–126.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E.
(2008) Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21, 481–504. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Taha, H.
(2017) The role of morphological and phonological awareness in the early development of word spelling and reading in typically developing and disabled Arabic readers. Dyslexia, 23(4), 345–371. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salverda, A. P., Brown, M., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(2011) A goal-based perspective on eye movements in visual world studies. Acta Psychologica, 137(2), 172–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salverda, A. P., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
(2017) The visual world paradigm. In A. M. B. de Groot, and P. Hagoort (Eds.), Research methods in psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language: A practical guide, (pp. 89–110). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Roth, E., & Ennemoser, M.
(2000) Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: A comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 284. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Speece, D. L., Roth, F. P., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S.
(1999) The relevance of oral language skills to early literacy: A multivariate analysis. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20(2), 167–190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J.
(2001) Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research, 65, 235–241. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C.
(2006) Eye movements and spoken language comprehension. In M. J. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (eds.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics, (2nd ed., pp. 863–900). New York: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tibi, S., & Kirby, J. R.
(2018) Investigating phonological awareness and naming speed as predictors of reading in Arabic. Scientific Studies of Reading, 22(1), 70–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar