Part of
Experimental Arabic Linguistics
Edited by Dimitrios Ntelitheos and Tommi Tsz-Cheung Leung
[Studies in Arabic Linguistics 10] 2021
► pp. 143176
References
Abdalla, F., & Crago, M.
(2008) Verb morphology deficits in Arabic-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(2), 315–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Abu-Allam, R., & Hadi, F.
(1998) Picture vocabulary test. Centre for Childhood, Evaluation, and Teaching, Kuwait.Google Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A.
(1977) A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorders III. Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(4), 383–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bartolucci, G.
(1982) Formal aspects of language in childhood autism. Advances in Child Behavior Analysis and Therapy, 2, 159–185.Google Scholar
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B.
(1998) Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1185–1192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brynskov, C., Eigsti, I. M., Jørgensen, M., Lemcke, S., Bohn, O. S., & Krøjgaard, P.
(2017) Syntax and morphology in danish-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(2), 373–383. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Van Der Lely, H. K. J.
(2008) Narrative discourse in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G.
(2003) Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language Impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(5), 1029–1037. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Crago, M., & Allen, S.
(2001) Early finiteness in Inuktitut: The role of language structure and input. Language Acquisition, 9, 59–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Jong, J.
(1999) Specific language impairment in Dutch: Inflectional morphology and argument structure. Enschede, The Netherlands: Print Partners Ipskamp.Google Scholar
Dunn, L., & Dunn, M.
(2007) PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Durrleman, S., & Zufferey, S.
(2009) The nature of syntactic impairment in autism. Rivista di Grammatica Generativa, 34, 57–86.Google Scholar
Durrleman, S., & Delage, H.
(2016) Autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment: Overlaps in syntactic profiles. Language Acquisition, 23(4), 361–386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrleman, S., Delage, H., Prévost, P., & Tuller, L.
(2017) The comprehension of passives in autism spectrum disorder. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2(1), 88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I. M., Bennetto, L., & Dadlani, M. B.
(2007) Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1007–1023. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I. M., & Bennetto, L.
(2009) Grammaticality judgments in autism: Deviance or delay. Journal of Child Language, 36(05), 999–1021. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, P., Leonard, L., Stokes, S., & Wong, A.
(2005) The expression of aspect in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 621–634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hansson, K., Nettelbladt, U., & Leonard, L.
(2000) Specific language impairment in Swedish: The status of verb morphology and word order. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 84–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Hobson, J. A.
(2010) Personal pronouns and communicative engagement in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(6), 653–664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Janke, V., & Perovic, A.
(2015) Intact grammar in HFA? Evidence from control and binding. Lingua, 164, 68–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2001) An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16 (2–3), 287–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L.
(2000) Specific language impairment across languages. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 115–129). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
(2014) Specific language impairment across languages. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 1–5. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L., Camarata, S., Brown, B., & Camarata, M.
(2004) Tense and agreement in the speech of children with specific language impairment: Patterns of generalization through intervention. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 1363–1379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, S., & Landau, B.
(2015) Language development in children with Williams syndrome: genes, modularity, and the importance of development. In E. Bavin and L. Naigles (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of child language, 2nd Edn. (pp. 609–636). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lombardi, L., & Potter, M.
(1992) The regeneration of syntax in short term memory. Journal of Memory And Language, 31(6), 713–733. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., & Paul, R.
(1997) Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive development disorders, 2nd ed. (pp. 195–225). New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., & Armon-Lotem, S.
(2015) Sentence repetition. Multilingual Matters, 1–32.Google Scholar
McCann, J., & Peppé, S.
(2003) Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Meir, N., & Novogrodsky, R.
(2019) Prerequisites of third-person pronoun use in monolingual and bilingual children with autism and typical language development. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Modyanova, N., Perovic, A., & Wexler, K.
(2017) Grammar is differentially impaired in subgroups of autism spectrum disorders: Evidence from an investigation of tense marking and morphosyntax. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morsi, R.
(2009) Specific language impairment in Egyptian Arabic A preliminary investigation. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Reading, UK.Google Scholar
Newcomer, P., & Hammill, D. D.
(2008) Test of language development: Primary, 4th edition. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Crago, M.
(2001) The morphosyntax of specific language impairment in French: An extended optional default account. Language Acquisition, 9, 269–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Park, C. J., Yelland, G. W., Taffe, J. R., & Gray, K. M.
(2012) Morphological and syntactic skills in language samples of pre-school aged children with autism: atypical development? International Journal of Speech-language Pathology, 14(2), 95–108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perovic, A., Modyanova, N., & Wexler, K.
(2013a) Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit? Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(4), 813–835. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perovic, A., Modyanova, N., Wexler, K., & Naigles, L. R.
(2013b) Comparison of grammar in neurodevelopmental disorders: The case of binding in Williams Syndrome and autism with and without language impairment. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 133–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pierce, S., & Bartolucci, G.
(1977) A syntactic investigation of verbal autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(2), 121–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poll, G. H., Betz, S. K., & Miller, C. A.
(2010) Identification of clinical markers of specific language impairment in adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 414–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Potter, M., & Lombardi, L.
(1990) Regeneration in the short-term recall of sentences. Journal Of Memory And Language, 29(6), 633–654. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1998) Syntactic Priming in Immediate Recall of Sentences. Journal Of Memory And Language, 38(3), 265–282. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., Tuller, L., Zebib, R., Barthez, M. A., Malvy, J., & Bonnet-Brilhault, F.
(2018) Pragmatic versus structural difficulties in the production of pronominal clitics in French-speaking children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism and Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 5–14. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H.
(2000) Manual for raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 3: The standard progressive matrices. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P.
(1995) Specific language impairments as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K.
(1996) Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in english-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1239–1257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rice, M., Noll, K., & Grimm, H.
(1997) An extended optional infinitive stage in German speaking children with specific language impairment. Language Acquisition, 6, 255–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2004) Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(3), 429–448. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Solari, E. J., Grimm, R. P., McIntyre, N. S., Zajic, M., & Mundy, P. C.
(2019) Longitudinal stability of reading profiles in individuals with higher functioning autism. Autism, 23(8). 1911–1926. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sundar Raj, M., Prasath, M., & Adalarasu, K.
(2015) Autism spectrum disorder. International Journal of Pharmacy and Technology, 7(2), 8638–8647. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Calkins, S.
(1990) Does imitation facilitate the acquisition of grammar? Evidence from a study of autistic, Down’s syndrome and normal children. Journal of Child Language, 17(3), 591–606. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H.
(1999) A psychological approach to understanding the social and language impairments in autism. International Review of Psychiatry, 11(4), 325–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Defining language impairments in a subgroup of children with autism spectrum disorder. Science China Life Sciences, 58(10), 1044–1052. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cooper, J.
(1999) Present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1261–1274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terzi, A., Marinis, T., Kotsopoulou, A., & Francis, K.
(2014) Grammatical abilities of greek speaking children with autism. Language Acquisition, 21(1), 4–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terzi, A., Marinis, T., & Francis, K.
(2016a) Syntax and its interfaces at the low and high ends of the autism spectrum. In A. M. Di Sciullo, (Ed.), Biolinguistic investigations on the language faculty (pp. 195–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016b) The interface of syntax with pragmatics and prosody in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 46(8), 2692–2706. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Terzi, A., Marinis, T., Zafeiri, A., & Francis, K.
(2019) Subject and object pronouns in high functioning children with ASD of a null-subject language. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X.
(1999) Language patterns and etiology in children with specific language impairment. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 361–382). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Tuller, L., Ferre, S., Prevost, P., Barthez, M., Malvy, J., & Bonnet-Brihault, F.
(2017) The effect of comuputational complexity on the acquisition of French by children with ASD. In L. R. Naigles (Ed.), Innovative investigations of language in autism spectrum disorder (pp. 115–140). New York, NY: American Psychological Association. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Innateness and learnability. In E. Bavin and L. Naigles (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of child language, 2nd Edn. (pp. 13–14). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E.
(1992) A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(3), 489–597. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K., & Chien, Y.-C.
(1985) The developemnt of lexical anaphor and pronouns. Paper and Report on Child Language Development, 24, 139–149.Google Scholar
Wittke, K., Mastergeorge, A. M., Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Naigles, L. R.
(2017) Grammatical language impairment in autism spectrum disorder: Exploring language phenotypes beyond standardized testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–12. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
World Health Orgnaization
(2013) Autism spectrum disorders and other developmental disorders: From raising awareness to building capacity. World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland, 1, 1–36. Retrieved from [URL]
Zhou, P., Crain, S., Gao, L., Tang, Y., & Jia, M.
(2015) The use of grammatical morphemesby mandarin-speaking children with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(5), 1428–1436. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Abd El-Raziq, Muna, Natalia Meir & Elinor Saiegh-Haddad
2023. Lexical skills in children with and without autism in the context of Arabic diglossia: Evidence from vocabulary and narrative tasks. Language Acquisition  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Antonijevic, Stanislava & Natalia Meir
2024. Cross‐Linguistic Perspectives on Morphosyntax in Child Language Disorders. In The Handbook of Clinical Linguistics, Second Edition,  pp. 259 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.