Implicit statistical learning and language acquisition
Experience-dependent constraints on learning
Acquiring spoken language involves implicitly learning the statistical relations among language units. In this chapter, we review recent behavioral and neurophysiological findings from our research group that illuminate the relation of this type of ‘implicit statistical learning’ (ISL) to language acquisition. First, we review evidence illustrating that ISL mechanisms enable the learner to predict upcoming language units. Second, we show modality constraints affecting the operation of ISL mechanisms, with auditory and visual learning biased to learn different types of patterns. Third, we demonstrate that under certain developmental conditions typical ISL abilities are altered, which can detrimentally affect subsequent language acquisition. These findings highlight the experience-dependent nature of ISL and its relation to typical and atypical language acquisition.
References
Altmann, G.T.M., Dienes, Z., & Goode, A
(
1995)
Modality independence of implicitly learned grammatical knowledge.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 899–912.


Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I
(
2012)
Statistical learning is related to reading ability in children and adults.
Cognitive Science, 36, 286–304.


Bauer, P.J., & Mandler, J.M
(
1992)
Putting the horse before the cart: The use of temporal order in recall of events by one-year-old children.
Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 441–452.


Bernstein, L.E
(
2005)
Phonetic processing by the speech perceiving brain. In
D.B. Pisoni &
R.E. Remez (Eds.),
Handbook of speech perception (pp. 79–98). Malden, MA: Blackwell.


Christiansen, M.H., Conway, C.M., & Onnis, L
(
2012)
Similar neural correlates for language and sequential learning: Evidence from event-related brain potentials.
Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 231–256.


Conway, C.M., Bauerschmidt, A., Huang, S.S., & Pisoni, D.B
(
2010)
Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key.
Cognition, 114, 356–371.


Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2009)
Seeing and hearing in space and time: Effects of modality and presentation rate on implicit statistical learning.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 561–580.


Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2006)
Statistical learning within and between modalities: Pitting abstract against stimulus-specific representations.
Psychological Science, 17, 905–912.


Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2005)
Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 24–39.


Conway, C.M., Deocampo, J., Walk, A.M., Anaya, E.M., & Pisoni, D.B
(
2014)
Deaf children with cochlear implants do not appear to use sentence context to help recognize spoken words.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2174–2190.


Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2001)
Sequential learning in non-human primates.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 539–546.


Conway, C.M., Goldstone, R.L., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2007)
Spatial constraints on visual statistical learning of multi-element scenes. In
D.S. McNamara &
J.G. Trafton (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 29th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.185–190). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Conway, C.M., Gremp, M.A., Walk, A.D., Bauernschmidt, A., & Pisoni, D.B
(
2012)
Can we enhance domain-general learning abilities to improve language function? In
P. Rebuschat &
J. Williams (Eds.),
Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 305–336). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.

Conway, C.M., Karpicke, J., Anaya, E.M., Henning, S.C., Kronenberger, W.G., & Pisoni, D.B
(
2011)
Nonverbal cognition in deaf children following cochlear implantation: Motor sequencing disturbances mediate language delays.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 237–254.


Conway, C.M., & Pisoni, D.B
(
2008)
Neurocognitive basis of implicit learning of sequential structure and its relation to language processing.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1145, 113–131.


Conway, C.M., Pisoni, D.B., Anaya, E.M., Karpicke, J., & Henning, S.C
(
2011)
Implicit sequence learning in deaf children with cochlear implants.
Developmental Science, 14, 69–82.


Conway, C.M., Pisoni, D.B., & Kronenberger, W.G
(
2009)
The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities: The auditory scaffolding hypothesis.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 275–279.


Coulson, S., King, J., & Kutas, M
(
1998)
Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations.
Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 21–58.


DeKeyser, R.M
(
1995)
Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17(3): 379–410.


Dye, M.W.G., & Bavelier, D
(
2010)
Attentional enhancements and deficits in deaf populations: An integrative review.
Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 181–192.

Eckerth, J., & Tavakoli, P
(
2012)
The effects of word exposure frequency and elaboration of word processing on incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition through reading.
Language Teaching Research, 16(2), 227–252.


Eden, G.F., Stein, J.F., Wood, H.M., & Wood, F.B
(
1995)
Temporal and spatial processing in reading disabled and normal children.
Cortex, 31, 451–468.


Eisenberg, L.S., Martinez, A.S., Holowecky, S.R., & Pogorelsky, S
(
2002)
Recognition of lexically controlled words and sentences by children with normal hearing and children with cochlear implants.
Ear & Hearing, 23(5), 450–462.


Elman, J.L
(
1993)
Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small.
Cognition, 48, 71–99.


Elman, J.L
(
1991)
Distributed representations, simple recurrent networks, and grammatical structure.
Machine Learning, 7, 195–225.


Emberson, L.L., Conway, C.M., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2011)
Timing is everything: Changes in presentation rate have opposite effects on auditory and visual implicit statistical learning.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 1021–1040.


Feldman, J., Kerr, B., & Streissguth, A.P
(
1995)
Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance.
Intelligence, 20, 87–114.


Fiser, J., & Aslin, R.N
(
2002)
Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants.
PNAS, 99(24), 15822–15826.


Gathercole, S.E., & Baddeley, A.D
(
1989)
Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study.
Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213.


Giroux, I. & Rey, A
(
2009)
Lexical and sublexical units in speech perception.
Cognitive Science, 33(2), 260–272.


Goldberg, A
(
1993)
Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 219- 224.


Goschke, T. & Bolte, A
(
2012)
On the modularity of implicit sequence learning: Independent acquisition of spatial, symbolic, and manual sequences.
Cognitive Psychology, 65, 284–320.


Hammill, D.D., Brown, V.L., Larsen, S.C., & Wiederholt, J.L
(
1994)
Test of adolescent and adult language: Assessing linguistic aspects of listening, speaking, reading, and writing (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Hawkins, J
(
2004)
On intelligence. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Griffin.

Hespos, S.J., Saylor, M.M. & Grossman, S.R
(
2009)
Infants ability to parse continuous actions.
Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 575–585.


Howard Jr., J.H., Howard, D.V., Japikse, K.C., & Eden, F.G
(
2006)
Dyslexics are impaired on implicit higher-order sequence learning, but not on implicit spatial context learning.
Neuropsychologia, 44, 1131–1144.


Hulme, C., & Snowling, M
(
1992)
Deficits in output phonology: An explanation of reading failure.
Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 47–72.


Hulstijn, J.H
(
2005)
Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129–140.


Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D
(
2012)
The best time to acquire new skills: Age-related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan.
Developmental Science, 15, 496–505.


Jones, J., & Pashler, H
(
2007)
Is the mind inherently forward looking? Comparing prediction with retrodiction.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 295–300.


Jost, E., Conway, C.M., Purdy, J.D., & Hendricks, M.A
(
2011)
Neurophysiological correlates of visual statistical learning in adults and children. In
L. Carlson,
C. Hoelscher, &
T.F. Shipley (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2526–2531). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Jusczyk, P., Houston, D.M., & Newsome, M
(
1999)
The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants.
Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159–207.


Kidd, E
(
2012)
Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax.
Developmental Psychology, 48, 171–184.


Kirkham, N.Z., Slemmer, J.A., & Johnson, S.P
(
2002)
Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism.
Cognition, 83, B35–B42.


Kirsner, K., Milech, D., & Standen, P
(
1983)
Common and modality-specific processes in the mental lexicon.
Memory & Cognition, 11(6), 621–630.


Klingberg, T
(
2010)
Training and plasticity of working memory.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324.


Krashen, S.D., & Terrell, T.D
(
1983)
The natural approach: Language acquisition in the classroom. San Fransisco, CA: The Alemany Press.

Kubovy, M
(
1988)
Should we resist the seductiveness of the space:time::vision:audition analogy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 318–320.


Kuhl, P.K
(
2004)
Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831–843.


Kveraga, K., Ghuman, A.S., & Bar, M
(
2007)
Top-down predictions in the cognitive brain.
Brain and Cognition, 65, 145–168.


Lam, W.Y.K
(
2009)
Examining the effects of metacognitive strategy instruction on ESL group discussions: A synthesis of approaches.
Language Teaching Resesarch, 13(2), 129–150.


Lashley, K.S
(
1951[2004])
The problem of serial order in behavior.
First language acquisition: The essential readings,
B. Lust &
C. Foley (Eds.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Leclercq, A., & Majerus, S
(
2010)
Serial order short term memory predicts vocabulary development: Evidence from a longitudinal study.
Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 417–427.


Manza, L., & Reber, A.S
(
1997)
Representing artificial grammars: Transfer across stimulus forms and modalities. In
D.C. Berry (Ed.),
How implicit is implicit learning? (pp. 73–106). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.


Mecklenbräuker, S., Hupbach, A., & Wippich, W
(
2003)
Age-related improvements in a conceptual implicit memory test.
Memory, 1208–1217.


Misyak, J.B., & Christiansen, M.H
(
2012)
Statistical learning and language: An individual differences study.
Language Learning, 62, 302–331.


Newport, E.L
(
1990)
Maturational constraints on language learning.
Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.


Perruchet, P., & Vinter, A
(
1998)
Parser: A model for word segmentation.
Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 246–263.


Poletiek, F.H., Conway, C.M., Ellefson, M.R., & Christiansen, M.H
under review).
Under what conditions can recursion be learned? Effects of starting small in artificial grammar learning of recursive structure.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.
Reber, A.S
(
1989)
Implicit learning and tacit knowledge.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(3), 219–235.


Reber, A.S
(
1967)
Implicit learning of artificial grammars.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 77, 317–327.

Reber, A.S
(
1993)
Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Rosenbaum, D.A., Cohen, R.G., Jax, S.A., Weiss, D.J., & van der Wel, R
(
2007)
The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy.
Human Movement Science, 26, 525–554.


Rüsseler, J., & Roesler, F
(
2000)
Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations.
Acta Psychologica, 104, 45–67.


Saffran, J.R
(
2001)
The use of predictive dependencies in language learning.
Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 493–515.


Saffran, J.R
(
2002)
Constraints on language learning.
Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 172–196.


Saffran, J.R., Aslin, R.N., & Newport, E.L
(
1996)
Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants.
Science, 274, 1926–1928.


Saffran, J.R., Johnson, E.K., Aslin, R.N. & Newport, E.L
(
1999)
Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults.
Cognition, 70, 27–52.


Schlaghecken, F., Stuermer, B., & Eimer, M
(
2000)
Chunking processes in the learning of event sequences: Electrophysiological indicators.
Memory & Cogniton, 28(5), 821–831.


Schmidt, R.W
(
1990)
The role of consciousness in second language acquisition.
Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 129–158.


Shamma, S
(
2001)
On the role of space and time in auditory processing.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 340–348.


Sharon, T., & Wynn, K
(
1998)
Individuation of action from continuous motion.
Psychological Science, 9, 357–362.


Robinson, C.W., & Sloutsky, V.M
(
2007)
Visual statistical learning: Getting some help from the auditory modality. In
D.S. McNamara &
J.G. Trafton (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 611–616). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Robinson, P
(
1997)
Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule-search, and instructed conditions.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 27–67.


Thomas, K., Hunt, R., Vizueta, N., Sommer, T., Durston, S., Yang, Y., & Worden, M.S
(
2004)
Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: An fMRI study of children and adults.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1339–1351.


Turk-Browne, N.B., & Scholl, B.J
(
2009)
Flexible visual statistical learning: Transfer across space and time.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 195–202.


Turk-Browne, N.B., Scholl, B.J., Chun, M.M., & Johnson, M.K
(
2009)
Neural evidence of statistical learning: Efficient detection of visual regularities without awareness.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1934–1945.


Walk, A.M., & Conway, C.M
(
2011)
Multisensory statistical learning: Can associations across perceptual categories be acquired? In
L. Carlson,
C. Hoelscher, &
T.F. Shipley (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3337–3342). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Zacks, J.M., Speer, N.K., Vettel, J.M., & Jacoby, L.L
(
2006)
Event understanding and memory in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Psychology & Aging, 21, 466–482.


Zacks, J.M., & Swallow, K.M
(
2007)
Event segmentation.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 80–84.


Cited by
Cited by 3 other publications
Andringa, Sible & Patrick Rebuschat
2015.
NEW DIRECTIONS IN THE STUDY OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT LEARNING.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 37:2
► pp. 185 ff.

Gerits, Hedwig & Kris Van den Branden
KIM, KATHY MINHYE & ALINE GODFROID
2019.
Should We Listen or Read? Modality Effects in Implicit and Explicit Knowledge.
The Modern Language Journal 
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 march 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.