Article published in:
Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages
Edited by Patrick Rebuschat
[Studies in Bilingualism 48] 2015
► pp. 325348
Cited by

Cited by 6 other publications

Abbas, Nawras, Tamar Degani & Anat Prior
2021. Equal Opportunity Interference: Both L1 and L2 Influence L3 Morpho-Syntactic Processing. Frontiers in Psychology 12 Crossref logo
Andringa, Sible & Patrick Rebuschat
2015. NEW DIRECTIONS IN THE STUDY OF IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT LEARNING. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 37:2  pp. 185 ff. Crossref logo
Hui, Bronson
2020. PROCESSING VARIABILITY IN INTENTIONAL AND INCIDENTAL WORD LEARNING. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 42:2  pp. 327 ff. Crossref logo
Li, Fengying, Chenlu Zhao & Weijian Li
2020. Implicit learning of semantic preferences of English words by Chinese learners. Consciousness and Cognition 84  pp. 102986 ff. Crossref logo
Shakouri, Nima & Parviz Maftoon
2016. The role of eye dominance in recalling lexical items in second language acquisition. International Journal of Research Studies in Language Learning 7:1 Crossref logo
Snefjella, Bryor, Nadia Lana & Victor Kuperman
2020. How emotion is learned: Semantic learning of novel words in emotional contexts. Journal of Memory and Language 115  pp. 104171 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 17 october 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.

References

References

Allopenna, P.D., Magnuson, J.S., & Tanenhaus, M.K.
(1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Allwright, R.
(1984) Why don’t learners learn what teachers teach? In D. Singleton & D. Little (Eds.), Language learning in formal and informal contexts (pp. 3–18). Dublin: IRAAL.Google Scholar
Altmann, G.T.M.
(2011) The mediation of eye movements by spoken language. In S.P. Liversedge, I.D. Gilchrist & S. Everling (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of eye movements (pp. 979–1003). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Andringa, S.J., & Curcic, M.
(2015) How explicit knowledge affects online L2 processing: Evidence from differential object marking acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(2) 237–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Baars, B.J., & Gage, N.M.
(2010) Cognition, brain, and consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. London: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baccino, T.
(2011) Eye movements and concurrent event-related potentials: Eye fixation-related potential investigations in reading. In S.P. Liversedge, I.D. Gilchrist & S. Everling (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of eye movements (pp. 857–870). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Batstone, R.
(2002) Contexts of engagement: A discourse perspective on “intake” and “pushed output”. System, 30, 1–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bax, S.
(2013) The cognitive processing of candidates during reading tests: Evidence from eye-tracking. Language Testing, 30(4), 441–465. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bax, S., & Weir, C.
(2012) Investigating learners' cognitive reading processes during a computer-based CAE Reading test. University of Cambridge ESOL Examinations Research Notes, 47(1), 3–14.Google Scholar
Bisson, M., Van Heuven, W.J.B., Conklin, K., & Tunney, R.J.
(2014) Processing of native and foreign language subtitles in films: An eye tracking study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(2), 399–418. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohan, J., & Sanford, A.J.
(2008) Semantic anomalies at the borderline of consciousness: An eye-tracking investigation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(2), 232–239. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, M., & Nazir, T.
(2005) Visual constraints in written word recognition: Evidence from the optimal viewing position effect. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(3), 216–228. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carr, T.H., & Curran, T.
(1994) Cognitive factors in learning about structured sequences: Applications to syntax. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 16, 205–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, C., Staub, A., & Rayner, K.
(2007) Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In R.P.G. V. Gompel, M.H. Fischer, W.S. Murray & R.L. Hill (Eds.), Eye-movements: A window on mind and brain (pp. 241–372). Oxford: Elsevier Science. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R.M.
(1974) The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(1995) Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., Lennertz, T., & Hannon, B.
(2007) Shallow semantic processing of text: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 83–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R.M.
(1995) Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 379–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2001) Automaticity and automatization. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 125–151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) Implicit and explicit learning. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 313–348). New York: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) What makes learning second-language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Language Learning, 55, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dienes, Z., & Scott, R.B.
(2005) Measuring unconscious knowledge: Distinguishing structural knowledge and judgment knowledge. Psychological Research, 69(5-6), 338–351. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P.E., & Sagarra, N.
(2007) The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 101–116. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P.E., Valdés Kroff, J.R., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) Visual world eye-tracking. In J. Jegerski & B. VanPatten (Eds.), Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 93–126). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dussias, P.E., Valdés Kroff, J.R., Guzzardo Tamargo, R.E., & Gerfen, C.
(2013) When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 353–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C.
(Ed.) (1994a) Implicit and explicit learning of languages. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(1994b) Implicit and explicit processes in language acquisition: An introduction. In N.C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 1–32). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ellis, N.C., Hafeez, K., Martin, K.I., Chen, L., & Boland, J.
(2012) An eye-tracking study of learned attention in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics. Crossref.Google Scholar
Ellis, R.
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141–172. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language. In R. Ellis, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Erlam, J. Philp & H. Reinders (Eds.), Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching (pp. 3–25). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., Loewen, S., Elder, C., Erlam, R., Philp, J., & Reinders, H.
(2009) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language learning, testing and teaching. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E.M., & Kliegl, R.
(2005) SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112(4), 777–813. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Felser, C., Cunnings, I., Batterham, C., & Clahsen, H.
(2012) The timing of island effects in nonnative sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(1), 67–98. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, L., & Rayner, K.
(1982) Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 178–210. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C.
(2005) Eye-movement recording as a tool for studying syntactic processing in a second language: A review of methodologies and experimental findings. Second Language Research, 21(2), 175–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C., & Pynte, J.
(1997) Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(1), 119–148. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S.M.
(1997) Input, interaction and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gass, S.M., & Mackey, A.
(2007) Data elicitation for second and foreign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Godfroid, A., Boers, F., & Housen, A.
(2013) An eye for words: Gauging the role of attention in incidental L2 vocabulary acquisition by means of eye-tracking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(3), 483–517. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Housen, A., & Boers, F.
(2010) A procedure for testing the Noticing Hypothesis in the context of vocabulary acquisition. In M. Pütz & L. Sicola (Eds.), Cognitive processing in second language acquisition (pp. 169–197). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Loewen, S., Jung, S., Park, J., Gass, S., & Ellis, R.
in press). Timed and untimed grammaticality judgments measure distinct types of knowledge: Evidence from eye-movement patterns. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(2.
(2015) Timed and untimed grammaticality judgments measure distinct types of knowledge: Evidence from eye-movement patterns. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(2), 269–297. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., & Schmidtke, J.
(2013) What do eye movements tell us about awareness? A triangulation of eye-movement data, verbal reports and vocabulary learning scores. In J.M. Bergsleithner, S.N. Frota & J.K. Yoshioka (Eds.), Noticing and second language acquisition: Studies in honor of Richard Schmidt (pp. 183–205). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai’i, National Foreign Language Resource Center.Google Scholar
Godfroid, A., & Uggen, M.
(2013) Attention to irregular verbs by beginning learners of German: An eye-movement study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 291–322. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Godfroid, A., Winke, P., & Gass, S.
(Eds.) (2013) Thematic issue on eye tracking in second language acquisition research, Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2).Google Scholar
Grosjean, F.
(1989) Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36, 3–15. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H.
(2012) The on-line integration of inflection in L2 processing: Predictive processing of German gender. In A.K. Biller, E.Y. Chung & A.E. Kimball (Eds.), BUCLD 36: Proceedings of the 36 th annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 226–245). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
(2013) Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research, 29(1), 33–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Huettig, F., Rommers, J., & Meyer, A.S.
(2011) Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica, 137, 151–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J.H.
(2003) Incidental and intentional learning. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acqusition (pp. 349–381). New York: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J.H., & Ellis, R.
(Eds.) (2005)  Implicit and explicit second-language learning . Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2). [Special issue] CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N.
(2007) Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57(1), 1–33. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jiménez, L.
(2003) Attention in probabilistic sequence learning. In L. Jiménez (Ed.). Attention and implicit learning (pp. 43–67). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jourdenais, R., Ota, M., Stauffer, S., Boyson, B., & Doughty, C.
(1995) Does textual enhancement promote noticing?: A think aloud protocol analysis. In R.W. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 183–216). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.Google Scholar
Juffs, A.
(2005) The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21(2), 121–151. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Keating, G.D.
(2009) Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59(3), 503–535. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2013) Eye-tracking with text. In J. Jegerski & B. VanPatten (Eds.), Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 69–92). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R.
(2006) Tracking the mind during reading: The influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 12–35. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, D.
(2010) Not so fast: A discussion of L2 morpheme processing and acquisition. Language Learning, 60, 221–230. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S.-K.
(2007) Effects of textual enhancement and topic familiarity on Korean EFL students' reading comprehension and learning of passive form. Language Learning, 57(1), 87–118. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2007) Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science, 33, 193–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lim, J.H., & Christianson, K.
(2014) Second language sensitivity to agreement errors: Evidence from eye movements during comprehension and translation. Applied Psycholinguistics. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G.D.
(2002) An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review, 109, 376–400. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meseguer, E., Carreiras, M., & Clifton, C.
(2002) Overt reanalysis strategies and eye movements during the reading of mild garden path sentences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 551–561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, D.C., Shen, X., Green, M.J., & Hodgson, T.L.
(2008) Accounting for regressive eye-movements in models of sentence processing: A reappraisal of the Selective Reanalysis hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(3), 266–293. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, J.M., & Ortega, L.
(2001) Does type of instruction make a difference? Substantive findings from a meta-analytic review. Language Learning, s1, 157–213. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pollatsek, A., Reichle, E.D., & Rayner, K.
(2006) Tests of the E-Z Reader model: Exploring the interface between cognition and eye-movement control. Cognitive Psychology, 52(1), 1–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K.
(1998) Eve movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Eye movements in reading: Models and data. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 2(5), 1–10.Google Scholar
Rayner, K., Sereno, S.C., Morris, R.K., Schmauder, A.R., & Clifton, C.
(1989) Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(3-4), SI21–SI49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rayner, K., Warren, T., Juhasz, B.J., & Liversedge, S.P.
(2004) The effect of plausibility on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30(6), 1290–1301. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reber, A.S.
(1993) Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J.N.
(Eds.) (2012) Statistical learning and language acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Reichle, E.D.
(Ed.) (2006)  Models of eye-movement control in reading . Cognitive Systems Research, 7(1). [Special issue]. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E.D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D.L., & Rayner, K.
(1998) Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125–157. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E.D., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K.
(2006) E-Z Reader: A cognitive-control, serial-attention model of eye-movement behavior during reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7(1), 4–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E.D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A.
(1999) Eye movement control in reading: accounting for initial fixation locations and refixations within the E-Z Reader model. Vision Research, 39, 4403–4411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003) The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 445–526. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reichle, E.D., Warren, T., & McConnell, K.
(2009) Using E-Z Reader to model the effects of higher level language processing on eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(1), 1–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reilly, R.G., & O'Regan, J.K.
(1998) Eye movement control during reading: A simulation of some word-targeting strategies. Vision Research, 38, 303–317. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R.
(1958) Validity of the Trial Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, L., Gullberg, M., & Indefrey, P.
(2008) Online pronoun resolution in L2 discourse: L1 influence and general learner effects. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(3), 333–357. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P.
(2003) Attention and memory during SLA. In C. Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 633–678). Oxford: Blackwell. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Cognitive abilities, chunk-strength, and frequency effects in implicit artificial grammar and incidental L2 learning: Replications of Reber, Walkenfeld, and Hernstadt (1991) and Knowlton and Squire (1996) and their relevance for SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 235–268. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, R., & Suh, B.-R.
(2007) Textually enhanced recasts, learner awareness, and L2 outcomes in synchronous computer-mediated interaction. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 197–227). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Ellis, N.C.
(2013) From seeing adverbs to seeing morphology. Language experience and adult acquisition of L2 tense. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 261–290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Seibert Hanson, A.
(2011) Eyetracking methodology: A user’s guide for linguistic research. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 4(2), 543–555. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, A.J., Leuthold, H., Bohan, J., & Sanford, A.J.S.
(2011) Anomalies at the borderline of awareness: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 514–523. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C., & Leow, R.P.
(Eds.) (2011) Implicit and explicit language learning: Conditions, processes, and knowledge in SLA and bilingualism. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R.
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 129–158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1995) Consciousness and foreign language learning: A tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (pp. 1–63). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai'i at Manoa.Google Scholar
(2001) Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction (pp. 3–32). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sharwood Smith, M.
(1991) Speaking to many minds: On the relevance of different types of language information for the L2 learner. Second Language Research, 7(2), 118–132. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shook, D.J.
(1994) FL/L2 reading, grammatical information, and the input to intake phenomenon. Applied Language Learning, 5, 57–93.Google Scholar
Simard, D., & Foucambert, D.
(2013) Observing noticing while reading in L2. In J.M. Bergsleithner, S.N. Frota & J.K. Yoshioka (Eds.), Noticing and second language acquisition: Studies in honor of Richard Schmidt (pp. 207–226). Honolulu, HI: National Foreign Language Resource Center, University of Hawai`i at Mānoa.Google Scholar
Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & Schmitt, N.
(2011) Adding more fuel to the fire: An eye-tracking study of idiom processing by native and nonnative speakers. Second Language Research, 27, 251–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & Van Heuven, W.J.B.
(2011) Seeing a phrase ‘time and again’ matters: The role of phrasal frequency in the processing of multi-word sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Language, Memory, and Cognition, 37(3), 776–784. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B.
(2010) Employing eye-tracking technology in researching the effectiveness of recasts in CMC. In F.M. Hult (Ed.), Directions and prospects for educational linguistics (pp. 79–97). New York, NY: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2012) Eye tracking as a measure of noticing: A study of explicit recasts in SCMC. Language Learning & Technology, 16(3), 53–81.Google Scholar
Spinner, P., Gass, S., & Behney, J.
(2013) Ecological validity in eye-tracking research: An empirical study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 389–415. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., Spivey-Knowlton, M.J., Eberhard, K.M., Sedivy, J.C.
(1995) Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tanenhaus, M.K., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2006) Eye movements and spoken language comprehension. In M. Traxler & M. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed.) (pp. 863–900). New York, NY: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trenkic, D., Mirkovic, J., & Altmann, G.T.M.
(2013) Real-time grammar processing by native and non-native speakers: Constructions unique to the second language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 237–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Truscott, J., & Sharwood Smith, M.
(2011) Input, intake, and consciousness: The quest for a theoretical foundation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(4), 497–528. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E.
(1983) Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
(2002) Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, G., Schmitt, N., & Galpin, A.
(2004) The eyes have it: An eye-movement study into the processing of formulaic sequences. In N. Schmitt (Ed.), Formulaic sequences: Acquisition, processing, and use (pp. 153–172). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Gompel, R.P.G., & Pickering, M.J.
(2007) Syntactic parsing. In M.G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 289–307). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vitu, F.
(2005) Visual extraction processes and regressive saccades in reading. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Cognitive processes in eye guidance (pp. 1–32). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vitu, F., & McConkie, G.W.
(2000) Regressive saccades and word perception in adult reading. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 301–326). Oxford: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Von der Malsburg, T., & Vasishth, S.
(2011) What is the scanpath signature of syntactic reanalysis? Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 109–127. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Warren, T., & McConnell, K.
(2007) Investigating effects of selectional restriction violations and plausibility violation severity on eye-movements in reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 770–775. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.N.
(1999) Memory, attention, and inductive learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21(1), 1–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 269–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009) Implicit learning in second language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 319–353). Bingley, UK: Emerald Press.Google Scholar
Winke, P.
(2013) The effects of input enhancement on grammar learning and comprehension: A modified replication of Lee, 2007 with eye-movement data. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 323–352. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Winke, P., Gass, S., & Sydorenko, T.
(2013) Factors influencing the reading of captions by foreign language learners: An eye-tracking study. The Modern Language Journal, 97(1), 245–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Winke, P., Godfroid, A., & Gass, S.
(2013) Introduction to the special issue: Eye-movement recordings in second language acqusition research. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(2), 205–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yang, S.-n., & McConkie, G.W.
(2001) Eye movements during reading: A theory of saccade initiation time. Vision Research, 41, 3567–3585. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Saccade generation during reading: Are words necessary? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 226–261. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) New directions in theories of eye-movement control during reading. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Cognitive processes in eye guidance (pp. 105–130). Oxford: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar