Part of
Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages
Edited by Patrick Rebuschat
[Studies in Bilingualism 48] 2015
► pp. 385416
References
Berry, D.C., & Broadbent, D.E
(1984) On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 209–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berry, D.C., & Dienes, Z
(1993) Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bolibaugh, C., & Foster, P
(2013) Memory-based aptitude for native-like selection: The role of phonological short-term memory. In G. Granena & M.H. Long (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 205–230). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, R.F., & D’Agostino, P.R
(1992) Stimulus Recognition and the Mere Exposure Effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 545–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braine, M., Brody, R., Brooks, P.D., Sudhalter, V., Ross, J.E., Catalano, L., & Fisch, S.M
(1990) Exploring language acquisition in children with a miniature artificial language: Effects of item and pattern frequency, arbitrary subclasses, and correction. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 591–610. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cleeremans, A., & Dienes, Z
(2008) Computational models of implicit learning. In R. Sun (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of computational modeling (pp. 396–421), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coppieters, R
(1987) Competence differences between natives and near-native speakers. Language 63, 544–573. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Graaff, R
(1997) The eXperanto experiment: Effects of explicit instruction on second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 249–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R.M
(1994) Implicit and explicit learning of L2 grammar: A pilot study. TESOL Quarterly, 28, 188–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1995) Learning second language grammar rules: An experiment with a miniature linguistic system. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 379–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 499–533.Google Scholar
(2003) Implicit and explicit learning. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp.313–348). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) What makes second-language grammar difficult? A review of issues. Language Learning, 55(Supplement 1), 1–25, DOI logoGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R.M., & Larson-Hall, J
(2005) What does the critical period really mean? In J.F. Kroll & A.M.B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 89–108). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dienes, Z. & Scott, R
(2005) Measuring unconscious knowledge: Distinguishing between structural and judgment knowledge. Psychological Research, 69, 338–351. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dimitrova-Vulchanova, M
(1998) Fragments of Balkan nominal structure. In A. Alexiadou & C. Wilder (Eds.), Possessors, predicates and movement in the determiner phrase (pp. 332–360). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Donaldson, B
(2011) Left-dislocation in near-native French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33(3), 399–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Doughty, C.J
(2003) Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 256–310). Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Durrant, P., & Schmitt, N
(2010) Adult learners’ retention of collocations from exposure. Second Language Research, 26(2), 163–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C
(1993) Rules and instances in foreign language learning: Interactions of explicit and implicit knowledge. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5, 289–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002) Frequency effects in language acquisition: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143–188.Google Scholar
(2005) At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305–352. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R
(2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(2), 141–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C
(2006a) Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning. Applied linguistics, 27(2), 164–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006b) Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C., & Cadierno, T
(2009) Constructing a second language. Introduction to the special section. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 111–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C., & Ferreira, F
(2009) Construction learning as a function of frequency, frequency distribution, and function. The Modern Language Journal, 93(3), 370–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N.C., & Larsen-Freeman, D
(2006) Language emergence: Implications for applied linguistic. Introduction to the special issue. Applied Linguistics, 27(4), 558–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Constructing a second language: Analyses and computational simulations of the emergence of linguistic constructions from usage. Language Learning, 59, 90–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Forster, K.I., & Forster, J.C
(2003) DMDX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, 35(1), 116–124. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldschneider, J., & DeKeyser, R.M
(2001) Explaining the 'natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition' in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants. Language Learning, 51, 1–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goo, J. & Mackey, A
(2013) The case against the case against recasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35, 1, 127–165 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gor, K., & Long, M.H
(2009) Input and second language processing. In W.C. Ritchie & T.J. Bhatia (Eds.). The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 445–472). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M.H
(2013) Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 311–343. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hama, M., & Leow, R.P
(2010) Learning without awareness revisited: Extending Williams (2005). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 32(3), 27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J.H
(2003) Incidental and intentional learning. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 349–381). London: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hyltenstam, K., & Abrahamson, N
(2003) Maturational constraints in SLA. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 539–588). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ioup, G., Boustagui, E., El Tigi, M., & Moselle, M
(1994) Reexamining the critical period hypothesis. A case study of successful adult SLA in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16(1), 73–98. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, Y., & Chun, M.M
(2003) Contextual cueing: Reciprocal influences between attention and implicit learning. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 277–296). Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karpicke, J.D., & Pisoni, D.B
(2004) Using immediate memory span to measure implicit learning. Memory and Cognition, 32, 956–964. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krashen, S.D
(1981) Principles and practice in second language acquisition. London: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Lambert, A.J., & Sumich, A.L
(1996) Spatial orienting controlled without awareness: A semantically-based implicit learning effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 490–518. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lambert, T
(2002) Visual orienting, learning and conscious awareness. In L. Jiménez (Ed.), Attention and implicit learning (pp. 253–275). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leow, R.P
(2000) A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behaviour. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 557–584. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leung, J. & Williams, J.N
(2006) Implicit learning of form-meaning connections. In R. Sun & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 465–470). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Leung, J.H.C., & Williams, J.N
(2011a) The implicit learning of mappings between forms and contextually derived meanings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(1), 33–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011b) Constraints on implicit learning of grammatical form-meaning connections. Language Learning, 62(2), 634–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, S
(2010) The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 309–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Long, M.H
(1991) Focus on form: A design feature in language teaching methodology. In K. de Bot, R.B. Ginsberg, & C. Kramsch (Eds.), Foreign language research in cross-cultural perspective (pp. 39–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1996) The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bahtia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413–68). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
(2003) Stabilization and fossilization in interlanguage development. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 487–535). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) Problems with supposed counter-evidence to the critical period hypothesis. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 43, 287–317. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Recasts in SLA: The story so far. In M.H. Long (Ed.), Problems in SLA (pp. 75–116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2009) Methodological principles for language teaching. In M.H. Long & C.J. Doughty (Eds.), Handbook of language teaching (pp. 373–94). Oxford: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Maturational constraints on child and adult SLA. In G. Granena & M.H. Long (Eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 3–41). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) A cognitive-interactionist theory of instructed SLA (ISLA). Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching (pp. 30–62). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Long, M., & Robinson, P
(1998) Focus on form: theory, research and practice. In C.J. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp.15–41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mackey, A
(2012) Input, interaction and corrective feedback in L2 learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., & Goo, J
(2007) Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 407–452). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marinova-Todd, S
(2003) Comprehensive analysis of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Harvard University, Graduate School of Education.
Meisel, J.M
(2011) First and second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newport, E.L
(2002) Critical periods in language development. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 737–39). London: Macmillan/Nature Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Norris, J.M., & Ortega, L
(2000) Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis. Language Learning, 50, 417–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J
(2009) Implicit learning of word order. Proceedings of the 31th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society . Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J.N
(2012) Implicit and explicit knowledge in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(04), 829–856. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, P
(2013) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge in second language research. Language Learning, 63(3), 595–626. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Russell, J. & Spada, N
(2006) Corrective feedback makes a difference: A meta-analysis of the research. In J. Norris & L. Ortega (Eds.), Synthesizing research on language learning and teaching (p. 133–164). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saffran, J.R., Newport, E.L., & Aslin, R.N
(1996) Word segmentation: The role of distributional cues. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 606–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saxton, M
(1997) The contrast theory of negative input. Journal of Child Language, 24, 139–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, R.W
(1990) The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 129–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1994) Implicit learning and the cognitive unconscious: Of artificial grammars and SLA. In N.C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 165–209). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Seger, C.A
(1994) Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 163–196. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stanley, W., Mathews, R., Buss, R., & Kotler-Cope, S
(1989) Insight without awareness: on the interaction of verbalization, instruction, and practice in a process control task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 41, 553–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Swain, M
(1991) French immersion and its offshoots: Getting two for one. In B. Freed (Ed.), Foreign language acquisition research and the classroom, 91–103. Lexington, MA: Heath.Google Scholar
Tagarelli, K., Borges-Mota, M., & Rebuschat, P
(2011) The role of working memory in implicit and explicit language learning. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , (pp. 2061–2066). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
Turner, M.L., & Engle, R.W
(1989) Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R.P., Schrock, J.C., & Engle, R.W
(2005) An automated version of the operation span task. Behavioral Research Methods, 37, 498–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Boxtel, S., Bongaerts, T., & Coppen, P.A
(2003) Native-like attainment in L2 syntax. In S. Foster-Cohen & S. Pekarek Doehler (Eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook (Vol. 3, pp. 157–181). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Boxtel, S
(2005) Can the late bird catch the worm? Ultimate attainment in L2 syntax. Utrecht: Landelijke Onderzoekschool Taalwetenschap (LOT).Google Scholar
Wan, L., Dienes, Z., & Fu, X
(2008) Intentional control based on familiarity in artificial grammar learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(4), 1209–1218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.N
(2004) Implicit learning of form-meaning connections. In B. VanPatten, J. Williams, S. Rott, & M. Overstreet (Eds.), Form meaning connections in second language acquisition (pp. 203–218). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2005) Learning without awareness. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 269–304. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Implicit learning in second language acquisition. In W.C. Ritchie & T.K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition. Bingley: Emerald.Google Scholar
Williams, J.N., & Kuribara, C
(2008) Comparing a nativist and emergentist approach to the initial stage of SLA: An investigation of Japanese scrambling. Lingua, 118, 522–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.N
(2010) Initial incidental acquisition of word order regularities: Is it just sequence learning?. Language Learning, 60(s2), 221–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, B., & Reber, A.S
(1994) Implicit learning and the acquisition of natural languages. In N.C. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages (pp. 115–145). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Yang, L.R., & Givón, T
(1997) Benefits and drawbacks of controlled laboratory studies of second language acquisition: The Keck second language learning project. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 173–193. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

KIM, KATHY MINHYE & ALINE GODFROID
2019. Should We Listen or Read? Modality Effects in Implicit and Explicit Knowledge . The Modern Language Journal 103:3  pp. 648 ff. DOI logo
Maie, Ryo & Robert M. DeKeyser
2020. CONFLICTING EVIDENCE OF EXPLICIT AND IMPLICIT KNOWLEDGE FROM OBJECTIVE AND SUBJECTIVE MEASURES. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 42:2  pp. 359 ff. DOI logo
Mostafaei Alaei, Mahnaz & Abbas Mansouri
2024. Unraveling the differential effects of task rehearsal and task repetition on L2 task performance: the mediating role of task modality. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 0:0 DOI logo
Mueller, Charles M
2018. Initial acquisition of tense-aspect morphology in an artificial language. Second Language Research 34:4  pp. 517 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 19 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.