Part of
Language Acquisition Beyond Parameters: Studies in honour of Juana M. Liceras
Edited by Anahí Alba de la Fuente, Elena Valenzuela and Cristina Martínez Sanz
[Studies in Bilingualism 51] 2016
► pp. 237279
References
Aguado, G.
(1988) Appraisal of the morphosyntactic competence in a 2.5 year old child. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 43, 75–95.Google Scholar
Alonso, A., & Henríquez Ureña, P.
(1964) Gramática castellana (Vol. 2). Buenos Aires: Losada.Google Scholar
Andersen, R.
(1984) What’s gender good for, anyway. In R. Andersen (Ed.), Second Languages: A Cross-linguistic Perspective (pp. 77–99). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Anderson, R.
(1999) Loss of gender agreement in L1 attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23, 319–338. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Archangeli, D.
(1984) Underspecification in Yawelmani Phonology and Morphology (Unpublished PhD dissertation). MIT.Google Scholar
Baker, P.
(2008) Sexed Texts: Language, Gender and Sexuality. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Hernandez, A., & Pizzamiglio, L.
(1996) Gender priming in Italian. Perception and Psychophysics, 58(7), 992–1004. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A.
(1985) The acquisition of Hebrew. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The Cross-linguistic Study of Language Acquisition, 1. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bhatia, T.K., & Ritchie, W.C.
(Eds.) (2004) The Handbook of Bilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Boloh, Y., & Ibernon, L.
(2010) Grammatical gender vs. natural gender in French Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1291–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boloh, Y., Escudier, F., & Royer, F.
(2012) Determiners and noun endings in French children’s gender attribution choices. First Language, 32(3), 398–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, D.
(2002) Adjectives, Number and Interfaces: Why Languages Vary. Oxford: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
(2005) Sériation des adjectives dans le SN et formation de concepts. Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes, 34, 125–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J. & White, L.
(2002) L2 acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In A. Pérez-Leroux & J. M. Liceras (Eds.), The Acquisition of Spanish Morphosyntax: The L1/L2 Connection (pp. 153–178). Dordrecht: Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bull, W. E.
(1965) Spanish for Teachers: Applied Linguistics. New York, NY: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Carroll, S.
(1989) Second language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning, 39, 535–594. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstens, V.
(2008) DP in Bantu and Romance. In C. De Cat & K. Demuth (Eds.), The Bantu-Romance Connection: A Comparative Investigation of Verbal Agreement, DPs, and Information Structure (pp. 131–166). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) Concord in minimalist theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 319–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Grammatical gender and the theory of uninterpretable features. In M. Putnam (Ed.), Exploring Crash-proof Grammars (pp. 31–57). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N.
(1993) A minimalist program for linguistic theory. In K. Hale & S. J., Keyser. (Eds.), The View from Building 20. Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 1–52). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(2001) Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (Ed.), K. Hale. A Life in Language (pp. 1–52). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G.
(1994) On the evidence for partial N-movement in the Romance DP. In G. Cinque, J. Koster, J. Pollock, L. Rizzi, & R. Zanuttini (Eds.), Paths towards Universal Grammar (pp. 85–110). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
(1995) Italian Syntax and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2010) The Syntax of Adjectives: A Comparative Study. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.
(1985) Language use and language users. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (3rd ed.; pp. 179–231). New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Corbett, G.
(1991) Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., & Quay, S.
(2000) Bilingual Acquisition: Theoretical Implications of a Case Study. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Epstein S. D., Kitahara, H., & Seely, T. D.
(2010) Uninterpretable features: What are they and what do they do? In M. Putnam (Ed.), Exploring Crash-proof Grammars (pp. 125–142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fantini, A. E.
(1985) Language Acquisition of a Bilingual Child: A Sociolinguistic Perspective. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press.Google Scholar
Fernández Fuertes, R., & Liceras, J. M.
(2010) Copula omission in the English developing grammar of English/Spanish bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(5), 525–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(dirs.) (2009) The FerFuLice Corpus: English/Spanish Bilingual Twins. [File data]. Available from [URL]
Fernández Ramírez, S.
(1986) Gramática española 4. El verbo y la oración. Madrid: Arco/Libros.Google Scholar
Foynard, M.
(2002) Etude de la production des déterminants chez des enfants francophones de 25 à 30 mois. Unpublished ms., Université Paris 5, France.Google Scholar
Franceschina, F.
(2001) Morphological or syntactic deficits in near-native speakers? An assessment of some current proposals. Second Language Research, 17, 213–247. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M.
(2002) Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In D. Kimbrough Oller & R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and Literacy in Bilingual Children (pp. 207–219). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gillis, S., & de Houwer, A.
(1998) Dutch child language: An overview. In S. Gillis & A. de Houwer (Eds.), The Acquisition of Dutch (pp. 1–100). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grégoire, A.
(1947) L’apprentissage du langage. Gembloux, France: J, Duculot.Google Scholar
Harris, J. W.
(1980) Nonconcatenative morphology and Spanish plurals. Journal of Linguistic Research, 1, 14–31.Google Scholar
(1983) Syllable Structure and Stress in Spanish. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
(1985) Spanish word markers. In F. H, Nuessel. Jr. (Ed.), Current Issues in Hispanic Phonology and Morphology (pp. 34–54). Bloomington, IN: Indiana Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
(1991) The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22(1), 27–62.Google Scholar
Hawkins, R., & Franceschina, F.
(2004) Explaining the acquisition and non-acquisition of determiner-noun gender concord in French and Spanish. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts. Focus on Functional Categories (pp. 175–205). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández Pina, F.
(1984) Teoría psicolingüísticas y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hickey, T.
(1991) Mean length of utterance and the acquisition of Irish. Journal of Child Language, 3, 553–569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulk, A.
(2004) The acquisition of the French DP in a bilingual context. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The Acquisition of French in Different Contexts, Focus. on Functional Categories (pp. 243–275). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jakubowicz, C. & Roulet, L.
(2008) Narrow syntax or interface deficit? Gender agreement in French SLI. In J. M. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The Role of Formal Features in Second Language Acquisition (pp. 184–225). New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1979) A Functional Approach to Child Language: A Study of Determiners and Reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kihm, A.
(2005) Noun class, gender, and the lexicon / syntax / morphology interfaces: A comparative study of Niger-Congo and Romance languages. In G. Cinque & R. Kayne (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Comparative Syntax (pp. 459–512). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Koehn, C.
(1994) The acquisition of gender and number morphology within NP. In J. Meisel (Ed.), Bilingual First Language Acquisition (pp. 29–51). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., Müller, N., & Cantone, K.
(2002) Gender inmonolingual and bilingual first language acquisition: Comparing Italian and French. Lingue e Linguaggio, 1, 107–149.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Sabbadini, L., Volterra, V., & Leonard, J.
(1988) Some influences on the grammar of English- and Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 39–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levy, Y.
(1983) It’s frogs all the way down. Cognition, 15, 75–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Díaz, L., & Mongeon, C.
(2000) N-drop and determiners in native and non-native Spanish: More on the role of morphology in the acquisition of syntactic knowledge. In CLAC: Círculo de lingüística aplicada a la comunicación, 3. Retrieved from [URL] (July 21, 2013).
Liceras, J. M., Fernández Fuertes, R., & Pérez-Tattam, R.
(2008) Null and over subjects in developing grammar (L1 English/L1Spanish) of two bilingual twins. In C. Pérez-Vidal, A. Bel, & M. Juan (Eds.), A Portrait of the Young in the New Multilingual Spain (pp. 111–134). Clevendon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Rosado, E., & Díaz, L.
(1998, September). On the differences and similarities between primary and non primary language acquisition: evidence from Spanish null nouns. Paper presented at EUROSLA ’98, The British Institute, Paris.
Lipski, J.
(1993) Creoloid phenomena in the Spanish of transitional bilinguals. In A. Roca & J. Lipski (Eds.), Spanish in the United States (pp. 155–173). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lleó, C.
(1998) Proto-articles in the acquisition of Spanish: Interface between phonology and syntax. In R. Fabri, A. Ortman, & T. Parodi (Eds.), Models of Inflection (pp. 175–195). Tübingen: Max Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López Ornat, S.
(1997) What lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? In W. R, Glass. & A. T. Pérez-Leroux (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish (pp. 3–20) . Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Lumsden, J.
(1992) Underspecification in grammatical and natural gender. Linguistic Inquiry, 23, 469–486.Google Scholar
Lumsden, J. S., & Halefom, G.
(2000) A Minimal Theory of Lexical Insertion: Synchronic and Diachronic Generalization in Syncretism and Suppletion (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Université du Québec á Montréal.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000) The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2009) The emergence of linguistic complexity. In T. Givon (Ed.), Linguistic Complexity (pp. 405–432). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Malakoff, M. E., Mayes, L. C., Schottenfeld, R., & Howell, S.
(1999) Language production in 24-month-old inner-city children of cocaine-and-other-drug-using mothers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, 159–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marantz, A.
(1997) No escape from syntax: Don’t try morphological analysis in the privacy of your own lexicon. In A. Dimitriadis (Ed.), Proceedings of the 21st annual Penn linguistics colloquium: Penn working papers in linguistics, 4(2), 201–225.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S.
(1998) El proceso de gramaticalización de las categorías nominales en español (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
(2008) Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: Starting small. Journal of Child Language, 35, 1–29.Google Scholar
Mel’cuk, I.
(1958) Statistika i zavisimost’ roda francuzskix suscestvitel’nyx ot ix okoncanija. Voprosy statistiki reci (pp. 112–30). Leningrad: Izdatel’stvo Leningradskogo Universiteta.Google Scholar
Miller, J. F., & Chapman, R. S.
(1981) The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24(2), 154–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mills, A. E.
(1986) The Acquisition of Gender. Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Monnery, S.
(2001) Calcul de l’attachement au sein du syntagme nominal: Le rôle des informations morphosyntaxiques de genre (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Université de Bourgogne.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K.
(2007) Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11, 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S.
(2008) Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58(3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N.
(1994) Gender and number agreement within DP. In J. Meisel (Ed.), Bilingual First Language Acquisition (pp. 53–88). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Namai, K.
(2000) Gender features in English. Linguistics, 38(4), 771–779. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Name, M. C. L. & Corrêa, L. M. S.
(2003) Delimitação perceptual de uma classe correspondente à categoria funcional D: Evidências da aquisição do Português. Fórum Lingüístico, 3(1), 55–88.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M.
(1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18, 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pesetsky, D., & Torrego, E.
(2007) The syntax of valuation and the interpretability of features. In S. Karimi, V. Samiian, & W. Wilkins (Eds.), Phrasal and Clausal Architecture (pp. 262–294). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pizzuto, E., & Caselli, M. C.
(1992) The acquisition of Italian morphology: Implications for models of language development. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 491–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radford, A., Uppish, T., Köppe, R., & Azzaro, G.
(2007) Concord, convergence and accommodation in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(3), 239–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roca, I. M.
(1989) The organisation of grammatical gender. Transactions of the Philological Society, 87, 1–32. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005) La gramática y la biología en el género del español (1ª parte). Revista Española de Lingüística, 35(1), 17–44.Google Scholar
(2006) La gramática y la biología en el género del español (2ª parte). Revista Española de Lingüística, 35(2), 397–432.Google Scholar
Rosado, E.
(1998) La adquisición de la categoría funcional determinante y los sustantivos nulos del español infantil (Unpublished PhD dissertation), University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Royle, P. & Valois, D.
(2007) The acquisition of concord in French and Spanish determiner phrases: Two elicitation experiments. Actas del XXIV Congreso Internacional de AESLA: Aprendizaje de lenguas, uso del lenguaje y modelación cognitiva: Perspectivas aplicadas entre disciplinas. Madrid: UNED.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Sadek, C., Kiraithe, J., & Villareal, H.
(1976) The acquisition of the concept of grammatical gender in monolingual and bilingual speakers of Spanish. In F. Aid, M. Resnick, & B. Saciuk (Eds.), Colloquium on Hispanic Linguistics (pp. 131–149). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Schiller, N. O., & Caramazza, A.
(2003) Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch. Journal of Memory & Language, 48, 169–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Serra, M., Sanz Torrent, M., Aguilar, E., Noguera, E., & Pañella, S.
(1999) Psycholinguistic profiles of Spanish specific language impairment children: Cognitive and linguistic difficulties from 3 to 4 years. Poster presented at the VIII International Congress for the Study of Child Language, San Sebastián.Google Scholar
Serra, M., Serrat, E., Solé, R., Bel, A. & Aparici, M.
(2000) La adquisición del lenguaje. Madrid: Ariel.Google Scholar
Serrat, E., Capdevilla, M. & Serra, M.
(1994) The interaction of functional categories and syntax in the acquisition of Spanish and Catalan. Paper presented at the first Lisbon meeting on child language, Lisbon.
Tanner, D.
(2008) Agreeing that agreement isn´t the problem. In M. Bowles, R. Foote, S. Perpiñán, & R. Bhatt (Eds.), Selected Proceedings of the 2007 Second Language Research Forum (pp. 22–34). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
Tucker, G. R., Lambert, W. E., & Rigault, A. A.
(1977) The French Speaker’s Skill with Grammatical Gender: An Example of Rule-governed Behavior. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska–MacGregor, M., & Yan-Kit I. L.
(2004) Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(1), 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 5 other publications

Balam, Osmer, Usha Lakshmanan & María del Carmen Parafita Couto
2021. Gender Assignment Strategies Among Simultaneous Spanish/English Bilingual Children from Miami, Florida. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 14:2  pp. 241 ff. DOI logo
Fernández Fuertes, Raquel, Tamara Gómez Carrero & Juana M. Liceras
2024. Activation and local inhibition in the bilingual child’s processing of codeswitching. Second Language Research DOI logo
Goebel-Mahrle, Thomas & Naomi L. Shin
2020. A corpus study of child heritage speakers’ Spanish gender agreement. International Journal of Bilingualism 24:5-6  pp. 1088 ff. DOI logo
Gómez Carrero, Tamara & Anastasiia Ogneva
2024. Who does it better?. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada/Spanish Journal of Applied Linguistics 37:1  pp. 143 ff. DOI logo
Keating, Gregory D.
2022. The Effect of Age of Onset of Bilingualism on Gender Agreement Processing in Spanish as a Heritage Language. Language Learning 72:4  pp. 1170 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.