Chapter published in:
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 7798
References

References

Alho, J. M.
(2008) Migration, fertility, and aging in stable populations. Demography, 45(3), 641–650. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(1), 3–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 229–235. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459–464. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. M. & Shafto, M. A.
(2008) Language and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373–443). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E. & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 1726–1729. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. A.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A. & Della Sala, S.
(2015) The importance of language use when studying the neuroanatomical basis of bilingualism. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L.M & Dunn, D. M.
(2007) The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Fourth Edition. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Schlichting, L.
(2005) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Pearson.Google Scholar
Festman, J. & Münte, T. F.
(2012) Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R.
(1975) “Mini-Mental State”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. & Lee, J.
(2010) Working memory capacity, inhibitory control, and proficiency in a second language. In M. S. Schmid & Lowie, W. (Eds.), Modeling bilingualism: from structure to chaos (pp. 59–84). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grant, D. A. & Berg E. A.
(1948) A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 404–411. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(2011) Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology 2011(2). CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grotjahn, R.
(1987) How to construct and evaluate a C-test: A discussion of some problems and some statistical analyses. Quantitative Linguistics, 23, 219–253.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R.
(2015) Why it is pointless to ask under which specific circumstances the bilingual advantage occurs. Cortex. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. C. & Curtiss, G.
(1993) Wisconsin card sorting test: manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Keijzer, M.
(2007) Last in first out? An investigation of the regression hypothesis in Dutch emigrants in Anglophone Canada. Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C. & Kempe, V.
(2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F. & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., de Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 588–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. & Frith, C. D.
(2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(8), 4398–4403. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Oller, J. W. & Streiff, V.
(1975) Dictation: A test of grammar-based expectancies. In R. L. Jones & B. Spolsky (Eds.), Testing language proficiency (pp. 71–82). Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Polk, T. A. & Farah, M. J.
(1995) Late experience alters vision. Nature, 376, 648–649 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. & Jarvis, S.
(2014) Lexical access and lexical diversity in first language attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 729–748. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Simon, J. R. & Wolf, J. D.
(1963) Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6(1), 99–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2010) Using magnitude estimation in developmental linguistic research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental methods in language acquisition research (pp. 57–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J. R.
(1935) “Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.”Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D. & Wodniecka, Z.
(2011) The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in psychology, 2. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tzelgov, J., Henik, A. & Leiser, D.
(1990) Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 16.5, 760–771.Google Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van den Noort, M. W. M. L., Bosch, P., Haverkort, M. & Hugdahl, K.
(2008) “A standard computerized version of the Reading Span Test in different languages.” European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24.1: 35–42. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A. & Duyck Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A. & Duyck, W.
(2016) Influence of language switching on the bilingual cognitive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 181-190 . CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A. & Grossman, M.
(2006) Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 2830–2839. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Liu, Minhua, Ingrid Kurz, Barbara Moser-Mercer & Miriam Shlesinger
2020. The interpreter’s aging. Translation, Cognition & Behavior 3:2  pp. 287 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 07 may 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.