Part of
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 7798
References
Alho, J. M.
(2008) Migration, fertility, and aging in stable populations. Demography, 45(3), 641–650. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(1), 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 229–235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burke, D. M. & Shafto, M. A.
(2008) Language and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 373–443). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E. & Freedman, M.
(2010) Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 1726–1729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. A.
(1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Bruin, A. & Della Sala, S.
(2015) The importance of language use when studying the neuroanatomical basis of bilingualism. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L.M & Dunn, D. M.
(2007) The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Fourth Edition. Bloomington, MN: NCS Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Schlichting, L.
(2005) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Pearson.Google Scholar
Festman, J. & Münte, T. F.
(2012) Cognitive control in Russian-German bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R.
(1975) “Mini-Mental State”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gass, S. & Lee, J.
(2010) Working memory capacity, inhibitory control, and proficiency in a second language. In M. S. Schmid & Lowie, W. (Eds.), Modeling bilingualism: from structure to chaos (pp. 59–84). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grant, D. A. & Berg E. A.
(1948) A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 404–411. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(2011) Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology 2011(2). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grotjahn, R.
(1987) How to construct and evaluate a C-test: A discussion of some problems and some statistical analyses. Quantitative Linguistics, 23, 219–253.Google Scholar
Hartsuiker, R.
(2015) Why it is pointless to ask under which specific circumstances the bilingual advantage occurs. Cortex. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. C. & Curtiss, G.
(1993) Wisconsin card sorting test: manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Keijzer, M.
(2007) Last in first out? An investigation of the regression hypothesis in Dutch emigrants in Anglophone Canada. Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C. & Kempe, V.
(2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F. & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., de Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 588–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. & Frith, C. D.
(2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(8), 4398–4403. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, J. W. & Streiff, V.
(1975) Dictation: A test of grammar-based expectancies. In R. L. Jones & B. Spolsky (Eds.), Testing language proficiency (pp. 71–82). Arlington, VA: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Polk, T. A. & Farah, M. J.
(1995) Late experience alters vision. Nature, 376, 648–649 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S. & Jarvis, S.
(2014) Lexical access and lexical diversity in first language attrition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(4), 729–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simon, J. R. & Wolf, J. D.
(1963) Choice reaction time as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6(1), 99–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2010) Using magnitude estimation in developmental linguistic research. In E. Blom & S. Unsworth (Eds.), Experimental methods in language acquisition research (pp. 57–72). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stroop, J. R.
(1935) “Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.”Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D. & Wodniecka, Z.
(2011) The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in psychology, 2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tzelgov, J., Henik, A. & Leiser, D.
(1990) Controlling Stroop interference: Evidence from a bilingual task. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 16.5, 760–771.Google Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van den Noort, M. W. M. L., Bosch, P., Haverkort, M. & Hugdahl, K.
(2008) “A standard computerized version of the Reading Span Test in different languages.” European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24.1: 35–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A. & Duyck Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A. & Duyck, W.
(2016) Influence of language switching on the bilingual cognitive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 181-190 . DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A. & Grossman, M.
(2006) Language and the aging brain: patterns of neural compensation revealed by functional brain imaging. Journal of Neurophysiology, 96(6), 2830–2839. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Liu, Minhua, Ingrid Kurz, Barbara Moser-Mercer & Miriam Shlesinger
2020. The interpreter’s aging. Translation, Cognition & Behavior 3:2  pp. 287 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.