Chapter published in:
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 99130
References

References

Axelrod, B. N., & Henry, R. R.
(1992) Age-related performance on the Wisconsin card sorting, similarities, and controlled oral word association tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 16–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 859–873.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bolla, K. I., Gray, S., Resnick, S. M., Galante, R., & Kawas, C.
(1998) Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 330–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Boone, K. B.
(1999) Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions: Impact of age, education, gender, intellectual level, and vascular status on executive test scores. In B. L. Miller & J. L. Cummings (Eds.), The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. The science and practice of neuropsychology series (pp. 247–260). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bopp, K. L., Verhaeghen, P.
(2005) Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 60B, 223–233. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brickman, A. M., Paul, R. H., Cohen, R. A., Williams, L. M., MacGregor, K. L., Jefferson, A. L., et al.
(2005) Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: Relationship to EEG theta power. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 561–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Branzi, F. M., Marne, P., Hernández, M., & Costa, A.
(2015) Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 65–78. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Branzi, F. M., & Costa, A.
(2011) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: Evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 399.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., MacWhinney, B., Flatt, M., & Provost, J.
(1993) PsyScope: An interactive graphical system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavioral Research Methods, Instrumentation and Computation, 25, 257–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M.
(2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491–511. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don‘t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossley, M., D'Arcy, C., & Rawson, N. S.
(1997) Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 52–62. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H.
(2001) Delis-Kaplan executive functioning system. San Antonio, TX: Psychology Corporation.Google Scholar
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I.
(2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R.
(1975) Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640–665.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Kleinman, D., & Wierenga, C. E.
(2014) What’s easier: Doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 2167–2195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Werner, G.
(2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P.
(2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–1164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Schotter, E. R., Gomez, J., Murillo, M., & Rayner, K.
(2014) Multiple levels of bilingual language control: Evidence from language intrusions in reading aloud. Psychological Science, 25, 585–595. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M.
(2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594–615. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goral, M., Campanelli, L., & Spiro III, A.
(2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 79–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T.
(1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 193–225. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Henry, J. D., Crawford, J. R., & Phillips, L. H.
(2004) Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1212–1222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernandez, A. E. & Kohnert, K. J.
(1999) Aging and language switching in bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6, 69–83. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Martin, C. D., Barceló, F., & Costa, A.
(2013) Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching? Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 257–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, D. & Gollan, T. H.
(submitted). Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches.
Kozora, E., Cullum, C. M.
(1995) Generative naming in normal aging: Total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 313–320. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ljungberg, J. K., Hansson, P., Andrés, P., Josefsson, M., & Nilsson, L.-G.
(2013) A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PLoS ONE, 8, e73029. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Lalonde, F., & Mack, C.
(1994) Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues:A double dissociations in normal subjects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1487–1492. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S.
(1988) Dementia rating scale: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Meuter, R. F., & Allport, A.
(1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A.
(1990) The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Portocarrero, J. S., Burright, R. G., & Donovick. P. J.
(2007) Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 415–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Poulisse, N.
(1999) Slips of the tongue: Speech errors in first and second language production. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A. & Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H.
(2013) The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer, The Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 622–645. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rascovsky, K., Salmon, D. P., Hansen, L. A., Thal, L. J., & Galasko, D.
(2007) Disparate letter and semantic category fluency deficits in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 21, 20–30. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, RM.
(1958) Validity of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. Perceptual Motor Skills, 8, 271–276. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Araujo, K., Weekes, V. A., Caracciolo, V., Padilla, M., et al.
(2000) Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish–English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 1–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A.
(1985) A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Sandoval, T.
(2010) The role of control in bilingual verbal fluency: Evidence from aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.Google Scholar
Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P.
(2010) What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency: The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 231–252. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Ruddell, A. P.
(1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O.
(2006) A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (Third Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tao, L, Taft, M. & Gollan, T. H.
(2015) The bilingual switching advantage: Sometimes related to bilingual proficiency, sometimes not. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 531–544. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L.
(1999) Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167–177.Google Scholar
Tomer, R., Levin, B. E.
(1993) Differential effects of aging on two verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 465–466. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Assche, E., Duyck, W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2013) Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1781–1792.Google Scholar
Van der Lubbe, R. & Verleger, R.
(2002) Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39, 100–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T.
(2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 780–789. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P.
(2011) Aging and executive control: Reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Psychological Science, 20, 174–180.Google Scholar
Verreyt, N., Woumans, E., Vandelanotte, D., Szmalec, A., & Duyck, W.
(2016) The influence of language switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 181-190. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., & Sliwinski, M.
(2011) Aging and task switching. A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 26, 15–20. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D.
(1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale (Third Ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959–974. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, R. T., & Hasher, L.
(1994) Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language (pp. 241–264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar