Chapter published in:
Growing Old with Two Languages: Effects of Bilingualism on Cognitive Aging
Edited by Ellen Bialystok and Margot D. Sullivan
[Studies in Bilingualism 53] 2017
► pp. 131159
References

References

Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. Q., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A.
(2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 2076–2086. Crossref link
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. Crossref link
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., Chaudhuri, J. R., & Kaul, S.
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. Crossref link
American Speech-Language Hearing Association
(1997) Guidelines for audiologic screening. Crossref link
Antón, E., García, Y. F., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A.
(2016) Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147–160. Crossref link
Astheimer, L. B., Berkes, M., & Bialystok, E.
(2016) Differential allocation of attention during speech perception in monolingual and bilingual listeners. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(2), 196–205. Crossref link
Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J.
(2014) Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75 (6), 959–963. Crossref link
Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V.
(2012) Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive Science, 36, 1129–1147. Crossref link
Bialystok, E.
(2005) Consequences of bilingualism for cognitive development. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
(2007) Cognitive effects of bilingualism: How linguistic experience leads to cognitive change. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 210–223. Crossref link
(2011) Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 229–235. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Abutalebi, J., Bak, T. H., Burke, D. M., & Kroll, J. F.
(2016) Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Research Reviews, 27, 56–60. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2010) Cognitive and Linguistic Processing in the Bilingual Mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 19–23. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Binns, M. A., Ossher, L., & Freedman, M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290–304. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M.
(2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia.Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2009) Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10, 89–129. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon Task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. Crossref link
Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859–73. Crossref link
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2007) Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 633–660. Crossref link
(2011) Bilingualism influences inhibitory control in auditory comprehension. Cognition, 118, 245–57. Crossref link
(2013) Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 547–567. Crossref link
(2014) Cognitive control in bilinguals: Advantages in stimulus-stimulus inhibition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 610–629. Crossref link
Brink, J., & McDowd, J.
(1999) Aging and selective attention: An issue of complexity or multiple mechanisms? The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54, P30–P33. Crossref link
Burke, D. M.
(1997) Language, aging, and inhibitory deficits: Evaluation of a theory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52B, P254–P264. Crossref link
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I.
(2006) How do highly proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? Inhibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32 (5), 1057–1074. Crossref link
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M.
(2010)  Delaying the onset of Alzheimer’s disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75, 1726–1729. Crossref link
De Leeuw, E., & Bogulski, C. A.
(2016) Frequent L2 language use enhances executive control in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19 (5), 907–913. Crossref link
Desroches, A. S., Newman, R. L., & Joanisse, M. F.
(2009) Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1893–906. Crossref link
Donohue, S., Liotti, M., Perez III, R., & Woldorff, M.
(2012) Is conflict monitoring supramodal? Spatiotemporal dynamics of cognitive control processes in an auditory Stroop task. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 1–15. Crossref link
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M.
(1997) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Dunn, L., Lugo, D., Padilla, E. & Dunn, D.
(1986) Test de Vocabulario en Imagenes Peabody. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service.
Durlik, J., Szewczyk, J., Muszyński, M., & Wodniecka, Z.
(2016) Interference and inhibition in bilingual language comprehension: Evidence from Polish-English interlingual homographs. PLoS ONE 11(3): e0151430. Crossref link
Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F.
(2010) Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6:5. Crossref link
Foldi, N., Helm-Estabrooks, N., Redfield, J., & Nickel, D.
(2003) Perseveration in normal aging: A comparison of perseveration rates on design fluency and verbal generative tasks. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10, 268–280. Crossref link
Freeman, M. R., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2016) Phonotactic constraints are activated across languages in bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(702), 1–12.
Freeman, M. R., Shook, A. & Marian, V.
(2016) Cognitive and emotional effects of bilingualism in adulthood. In E. Nicoladis, & S. Montanari (Eds.), Lifespan Perspectives on Bilingualism. Berlin, Germany: APA and De Gruyter. Crossref link
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez-Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., Hernandez, M., et al.
(2010) Bridging language and attention: brain basis of the impact of bilingualism on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53, 1272–1278. Crossref link
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1993) Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanisms. Psychological Science, 4, 294–298. Crossref link
Gernsbacher, M., & Faust, M.
(1991) The mechanism of suppression: A component of general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245–262.
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kriscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D.
(2013) Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387–396. Crossref link
Gollan, T. H., & Ferreira, V. S.
(2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640.
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C.
(2008) More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. Crossref link
Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P.
(2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–64. Crossref link
Goral, M., Libben, G., Obler, L. K., Jarema, G., & Ohayon, K.
(2008) Lexical attrition in younger and older bilingual adults. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 22, 509–522. Crossref link
Gorfein, D. S., & Brown, V. R.
(2007) Saying no to inhibition: the encoding and use of words. In D. S. Gorfein & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.). The Place of Inhibition in Cognition.Washington: APA Books. Crossref link
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. Crossref link
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 515–530. Crossref link
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T.
(1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 22 (pp. 193–225). New York: Academic Press. Crossref link
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 625–658. Crossref link
Ihle, A., Oris, M., Fagot, D., & Kliegel, M.
(2016) The relation of the number of languages spoken to performance in different cognitive abilities in old age. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1–12.
Ivanova, I., Murillo, M., Montoya, R. I., & Gollan, T. H.
(2016) Does bilingual language control decline in older age? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(1/2), 87–117.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W.
(2003) Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47.
Kaushanskaya, M., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V.
(2011) The relationship between short-term memory and vocabulary measures in monolingual and bilingual speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (4), 408–425. Crossref link
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V.
(2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 1–9. Crossref link
Kornblum, S.
(1994) The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop-and Simon-like stimuli. Psychological Research, 56, 130–135. Crossref link
Kovács, A., & Mehler, J.
(2009) Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. doiCrossref link
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Marian, V., & Kraus, N.
(2014) Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and Language, 128, 34–40. Crossref link
Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychological Science Agenda, American Psychological Association, 22.
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. Crossref link
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T.
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech: Evidence for inhibitory processes. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416–430. Crossref link
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes Kroff, J. R.
(2012) Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 56 (pp. 229–262). Crossref link
Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 349.
Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L.
(2004) Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. NeuroImage, 22, 1097–1106. Crossref link
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B.
(1998) Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1–36. Crossref link
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. Crossref link
Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T.
(2007) Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a “new view.” Inhibition in Cognition, 145–162. Crossref link
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M.
(2007) The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50 (4), 940–967. Crossref link
Marian, V., Chabal, S., Bartolotti, J., Bradley, K., & Hernandez, A.
(2014) Differential recruitment of executive control regions during phonological competition in monolinguals and bilinguals. Brain and Language. Crossref link.
Marian, V., & Spivey, M.
(2003) Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97–115. Crossref link
Martín, M. C., Macizo, P., & Bajo, T.
(2010) Time course of inhibitory processes in bilingual language processing. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 679–693. Crossref link
Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(1987) Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71–102. Crossref link
Mason, R., & Just, M.
(2007) Lexical ambiguity in sentence comprehension. Brain Research, 1146, 115–127. Crossref link
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L.
(1986) The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–86. Crossref link
Mercier, J., Pivneva, I., & Titone, D.
(2014) Individual differences in inhibitory control relate to bilingual spoken word processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 89–117. Crossref link
Mirman, D.
(2014) Growth Curve Analysis and Visualization Using R. Chapman and Hall / CRC.
Mishra, R., Hilchey, M., Singh, N., & Klein, R.
(2012) On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: Higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1502–1510. Crossref link
Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gómez-Ariza, C. J., & Bajo, M. T.
(2015) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. Crossref link
Paap, K. R. I., & Greenberg, Z.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. Crossref link
Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. Crossref link
PsychCorp
(1999) Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Revill, K. P., & Spieler, D. H.
(2012) The effect of lexical frequency on spoken word recognition in young and older listeners. Psychology and Aging, 27, 80–7. Crossref link
Salthouse, T. A.
(1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428. Crossref link
Salthouse, T. A., & Meinz, E. J.
(1995) Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Journal of Gerontology, 50B, 297–306. Crossref link
Seidenberg, M. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Leiman, J. M., & Bienkowski, M.
(1982) Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 489–537. Crossref link
Singh, N., & Mishra, R.
(2014) The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: Evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1481.
Swinney, D.
(1979) Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re) consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 645–659 Crossref link
Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Mishler, A., Corbett, R., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Trueswell, J. C., & Novick, J. M.
(2016) The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition, 150, 213–231. Crossref link
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
(1999) Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959.
West, R. & Baylis, G.
(1998) Effects of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 13, 206–217. Crossref link
Wingfield, A.
(1996) Cognitive factor in auditory performance: context, speed of processing, and constraints of memory. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 7, 175–182.
Wingfield, A., Amichetti, N. M., & Lash, A.
(2015) Cognitive aging and hearing acuity: modeling spoken language comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. Crossref link