Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 117130
References
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W.
(2016) Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals: neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. R.
(1992) A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H.
(2008) Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Birdsong, D.
(2005) Interpreting age effects in second language acquisition. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp. 109–127). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2009) Age and the end state of second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 401–424). Bingley: Emerald.Google Scholar
Bozic, M., Tyler, L. K., Su, L., Wingfield, C., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(2013) Neurobiological systems for lexical representation and analysis in English. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1678–1691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dekydtspotter, L., & Miller, A. K.
(2013) Inhibitive and facilitative priming induced by traces in the processing of wh-dependencies in a second language. Second Language Research, 29, 345–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diependaele, K., Duñabeitia, J. A., Morris, J., Keuleers, E.
(2011) Fast morphological effects in first and second language word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 344–358. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R.
(1999) Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., O’Connor, P. A., & Moscoso Del Prado Martín, F.
(2009) Early morphological processing is morphosemantic and not simply morpho-orthographic: A violation of form-then-meaning accounts of word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 684–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frost, R., Deutsch, A., Gilboa, O., Tannenbaum, M., Marslen-Wilson, W.
(2000) Morphological priming: Dissociation of phonological, semantic, and morphological factors. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1277–1288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldrick, M., Putnam, M., & Schwarz, L.
(2016) Coactivation in bilingual grammars: A computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.Google Scholar
Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E.
(2003) Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 14, 31–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R.
(2015) Why it is pointless to ask under which specific circumstances the bilingual advantage occurs. Cortex73, 336–337. 15 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heyer, V., & Clahsen, H.
(2015) Late bilinguals see a scan in scanner AND in scandal: Dissecting formal overlap from morphological priming in the processing of derived words. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 543–550. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacob, G., Heyer, V., & Veríssimo, J.
(2018) Aiming at the same target: A masked priming study directly comparing derivation and inflection in the second language. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 619–637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., Newport, E. L.
(1989) Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. & Rodriguez, G. A.
(2015) Second language sentence processing. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kirkici, B., & Clahsen, H.
(2013) Inflection and derivation in native and non-native language processing: Masked priming experiments on Turkish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 776–794. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liang, L., & Chen, B.
(2014) Processing morphologically complex words in second-language learners: The effect of proficiency. Acta Psychologica, 150, 69–79. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Linck, J. A., Hoshino, N., & Kroll, J. F.
(2008) Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. Mental Lexicon, 3, 349–374. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., de Sa, E., & Bialystok, E.
(2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 588–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
(2007) Morphological processes in language comprehension. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 175–193). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D., Bozic, M., & Randall, B.
(2008) Early decomposition in visual word recognition: Dissociating morphology, form, and meaning. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 394–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L.
(2013) Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 313–325.Google Scholar
Pinker, S.
(1994) The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Morrow. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raveh, M., and Rueckl, J. G.
(2000) Equivalent effects of inflected and derived primes: Long-term morphological priming in fragment completion and lexical decision. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B.
(2004) The broth in my brother’s brothel: morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1090–1098. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva, R. & Clahsen, H.
(2008) Morphologically complex words in L1 and L2 processing: Evidence from masked priming experiments in English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 245–260. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stanners, R. F., Neisser, J. J., Hernon, W. P., & Hall, R.
(1979) Memory representation for morphologically related words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 399–412. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stump, G.
(2001) Inflectional morphology: A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vanhove, J.
(2013) The critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition: A statistical critique and a reanalysis. PLoS ONE, 8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J. & Clahsen, H.
(2009) Morphological priming by itself: A study of Portuguese conjugations. Cognition, 112, 187–194. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veríssimo, J., Heyer, V., Jacob, G., & Clahsen, H.
(2018) Selective effects of age of acquisition on morphological priming: Evidence for a sensitive period. Language Acquisition, 25, 315–326. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voga, M., Anastassiadis-Syméonidis, A., & Giraudo, H.
Xu, Y.
(2014) Processing relative clauses in Chinese as a second language. Second Language Research, 30, 439–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar