Chapter published in:
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 147159
References

References

Barac, R. & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83(2), 413–422.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2013) Package ‘lme4’.Google Scholar
Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 931–49. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1988) Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 560–567. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. & Martin, M. M.
(2004) Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325–339. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K.
(1986) Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P.
(2007) Syntactic priming. Language and Linguistic Compass, 1(1–2), 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Jones, M. G.
(2005) A blue cat or a cat that is blue? Evidence for abstract syntax in young children’s noun phrases. In A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha, The proceedings of the 29th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 109–121). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Cai, Z., Pickering, M., & Branigan, H. P.
(2011) Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-Mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 421–445. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science 11(2), 282–298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cattaneo, G., Calabria, M., Marne, P., Gironell, A., Abutalebi, J., & Costa, A.
(2015) The role of executive control in bilingual language production: A study with Parkinson’s Disease patients. Neuropsychologia, 66, 99–110. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, Lloyd M., Dunn, Leota M., Wheton, C., & Burtlet, J.
(2007) The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). London: GL Assessment Limited.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The Adaptive Control Hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E.
(2004) Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 6(15), 409–414. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hervé, C., Serratrice, L., & Corley, M.
(2015) Dislocations in French-English bilingual children: An elicitation study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 987–1000. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497–514. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lyster Haalas, S. A., Horn, E., & Rygvold, A. E.
(2010) Ordforråd og ordforråd utvikling hos Norske barn og unge. Resultater fra en utprøving av British Picture Vocabulary Scale II (2nd ed.) (BPVS II). Spesialpedagogikk, 74(9), 35–43.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P.
(2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Gómez-Ariza, C., & Bajo, M. T.
(2013) Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 531–546. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mukadam, N., Sommerland, A., & Livingston, G.
(2017) The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 58(1), 45–54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicolay, A. C. & Poncelet, M.
(2013) Cognitive abilities underlying second-language vocabulary acquisition in an early second-language immersion education context: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 655–671. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., Sorace, A., & Tsapkini, K.
(2017) Language interference and inhibition in early and late successive bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J.
(2007) The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(2), 153–171. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T.
(1988) From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2016) Language and cognition in bilingual production: The real work still lies ahead. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tungseth, M. E.
(2006) Verb preposition in Norwegian: Paths, places and possession (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UiT, the Arctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2017) Bilingual language experience calculator. Nijmegen: Radboud University Nijmegen.Google Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P.
(2010) Cross-linguistic structural priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 37, 1047–1064.CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wolleb, A.
(2015) Syntactic representations in the bilingual mind: The role of executive function and pragmatics in cross-language priming (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). UiT, the Artctic University of Norway.Google Scholar
Wolleb, A., Sorace, A., & Westergaard, M.
(2017) Exploring the role of cognitive control in syntactic processing: Evidence from cross-language priming in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., & Frye, D.
(1998) Cognitive complexity and control: The development of executive function in childhood. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 4(7), 121–126. CrossrefGoogle Scholar