Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 161180
References
Abutalebi, J., Green, D. W.
(2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 557–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Backus, A.
(2015) A usage-based approach to codeswitching: The need for reconciling structure and function. In: G. Stell, K. Yapko eds. (Eds). Code-switching at the crossroads between structural and sociolinguistic perspectives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Bhatt, R., Bolonyai, A.
(2011) Code-switching and the optimal grammar of bilingual language use. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 522–546. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Global-Local and Trail-Making Tasks by Monolingual and Bilingual children: Beyond Inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46, 93–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. G., Luk, G.
(2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for Mind and Brain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 240–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D.
(2001) Conflict monitoring and Cognitive Control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clyne, M.
(2003) Dynamics of Language Change. Cambridge Approaches to Language Contact, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., Sebastian-Galles, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. M. B., Christoffels, I. K.
(2006) Language Control in bilinguals: Monolingual tasks and simultaneous interpreting. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 189–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deuchar, M., Muysken, P. & Wang, S.
(2008) Structured Variation in Codeswitching: Towards an Empirically Based Typology of Bilingual Speech Patterns. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 298–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eppler, E.
(2005) The Syntax of German-English code-switching. PhD Thesis, UCL.Google Scholar
Gardner-Chloros, P.
(2009) Code-switching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W.
(1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language Control in Bilinguals: The Adaptive Control Hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., Wei, L.
(2014) A control process model of CS. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 24, 499–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., Kroll, J. F.
(2011) Local and global inhibition in bilingual word production: fMRI evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals. Neuroimage, 56, 2300–2309. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., Puls, B.
(2013) Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 673–680. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Yudes, C., Gomez-Ariza, C. J., Teresa Bajo, M.
(2015) Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 66, 157–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Muysken, P.
(2000) Bilingual Speech: A Typology of Code-mixing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing, Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prior, A., Gollan, T. H.
(2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H.
(1998) Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within-group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 183.Google Scholar
Treffers-Daller, J.
(1998) The IC model and code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 98–99.Google Scholar
(2009) Code-switching and transfer: an exploration of similarities and differences. In: Bullock, B. E. and Almeida, J. T. (Eds). The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Codeswitching. Cambridge University Press, pp. 58–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Markiewicz, Roksana, Ali Mazaheri & Andrea Krott
2023. Bilingualism can cause enhanced monitoring and occasional delayed responses in a flanker task. European Journal of Neuroscience 57:1  pp. 129 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 23 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.