Part of
Bilingualism, Executive Function, and Beyond: Questions and insights
Edited by Irina A. Sekerina, Lauren Spradlin and Virginia Valian
[Studies in Bilingualism 57] 2019
► pp. 281293
References
Bialystok, E.
(2010) Global–local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 93–105. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S.
(2010) Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K.
(1990) The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10(1), 48–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, K. A., Chatham, C. H., Wiseheart, M., & Munakata, Y.
(2014) A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: The case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds. Neuropsychologia, 62, 356–364. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S.
(2011) Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borella, E., Carretti, B., & Pellegrina, S.
(2010) The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43, 541–552. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D.
(2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brenhouse, H. C., & Andersen, S. L.
(2011) Developmental trajectories during adolescence in males and females: A cross-species understanding of underlying brain changes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(8), 1687–1703. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. J.
(1988) Task complexity: A review and analysis. Academy of Management Review, 13(1), 40–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cepeda, N. J., Blackwell, K. A., & Munakata, Y.
(2013) Speed isn’t everything: Complex processing speed measures mask individual differences and developmental changes in executive control. Developmental Science, 16(2), 269–286. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chiaravalloti, N. D., Christodoulou, C., Demaree, H. A., & DeLuca, J.
(2003) Differentiating simple versus complex processing speed: Influence on new learning and memory performance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(4), 489–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. D.
(2017) Cognitive control. Core constructs and current considerations. In T. Egner (Ed.). The Wiley handbook of cognitive control (pp. 3–28). Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M.
(2011) Post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demetriou, A.
(2002) Tracing psychology’s invisible giant and its visible guards. In. R. J. Sternber & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.). The general factor of intelligence. How general is it? (pp. 3–18). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G., Shayer, M., Mouyi, A., Kazi, S., & Platsidou, M.
(2013) Cycles in speed-working memory-G relations: Towards a developmental–differential theory of the mind. Intelligence, 41(1), 34–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dodwell, K., & Bavin, E. L.
(2008) Children with specific language impairment: An investigation of their narratives and memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 201–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Egner, T.
(Ed.) (2017) The Wiley handbook of cognitive control. Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A.
(2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grudnik, J. L., & Kranzler, J. H.
(2001) Meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence, 29(6), 523–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G.
(2012) Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–45. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S.
(2013) Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 884–899. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J.
(2006) Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R.
(2006) Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Kail, R.
(1991b) Development of processing speed in childhood and adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 23, 151–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., & Park, Y. S.
(1990) Impact of practice on speed of mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49(2), 227–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. V.
(2008) Speed of processing in childhood and adolescence: Nature, consequences, and implications for understanding atypical development. In J. DeLuca & J. H. Kalmar (Eds.), Information Processing Speed in Clinical Populations (pp. 101–123). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis,Google Scholar
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A.
(1994) Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86(2), 199–225. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 497–514. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L.
(2015) Bilingualism, mind, and brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 377–394. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E.
(2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K.
(2016) Executive control in bilingual children: Factors that influence the outcomes. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(5), 575–589. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Eichorn, N., Scheuer, J., & Yoon, J.
(2014) Information processing and proactive interference in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 106–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Scheuer, J., Yoon, J., & Eichorn, N.
(2012) Executive function profiles in children with and without specific language impairment. Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata/Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 12(3), 9–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marton, K., Goral, M., Campanelli, L., Yoon, J., & Obler, L. K.
(2017) Executive control mechanisms in bilingualism: Beyond speed of processing. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 20(3), 613–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCrory, C., & Cooper, C.
(2005) The relationship between three auditory inspection time tasks and general intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(8), 1835–1845. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187–202. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Notebaert, W., Houtman, F., Van Opstal, F., Gevers, W., Fias, W., & Verguts, T.
(2009) Post-error slowing: an orienting account. Cognition, 111(2), 275–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Posthuma, D. & de Geus, E.
(2008) The genetics of information processing speed in humans. In. J. DeLuca & J. H. Kalmar (Eds.), Information processing speed in clinical populations (pp. 79–100). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Roth, C.
(1983) Factors affecting developmental changes in the speed of processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35(3), 509–528. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M.
(2011) The effect of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 26–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sheridan, H., & Reingold, E. M.
(2014) Expert vs. novice differences in the detection of relevant information during a chess game: evidence from eye movements. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–6. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W.
(1977) Controlled and automatic information processing, II: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84, 127–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valian, V.
(2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(1), 3–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voss, A., Nagler, M., & Lerche, V.
(2013) Diffusion models in experimental psychology. Experimental Psychology, 60(6), 385–402. DOI logoGoogle Scholar