Chapter published in:
Developing Narrative Comprehension: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives
Edited by Ute Bohnacker and Natalia Gagarina
[Studies in Bilingualism 61] 2020
► pp. 270296
References

References

Aldrich, N. J., & Brooks, P. J.
(2017) Linguistic and socio-cognitive predictors of school-age children’s narrative evaluations about jealousy. First Language, 37(2), 130–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R.
(1994) Role of reader’s schema in comprehension, learning and memory. In R. Ruddell, M. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (4th ed., pp. 469–482). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., de Jong, J., & Meir, N.
(Eds.) (2015) Assessing multilingual children. Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beltz, A. M., Blakemore, J. E. O. & Berenbaum, S. A.
(2013) Sex differences in brain and behavioral development. In H. Tager-Flusberg, P. Rakic, & J. Rubenstein (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience, Vol. 3: Neural circuit development and function in the healthy and diseased brain (pp. 467–499). Oxford: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I.
(Eds.) (1994) Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bildungsberichterstattung
(2018) Bildung in Deutschland 2018. Ein indikatorengestützter Bericht mit einer Analyse zu Wirkungen und Erträgen von Bildung. Bielefeld: wbv media.Google Scholar
Boerma, T., Leseman, P., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F., & Blom, E.
(2016) Narrative abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment: Implications for clinical practice. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51(6), 626–638. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohnacker, U.
(2016) Tell me a story in English or Swedish: Narrative production and comprehension in bilingual preschoolers and first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 19–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bohnacker, U., & Lindgren, J.
in press). MAIN story comprehension: What can we expect of a typically developing child? In S. Armon-Lotem & K. Grohmann Eds. LITMUS in action: Cross-comparison studies across Europe Amsterdam John Benjamins Preprint retrieved from http://​uu​.diva​-portal​.org​/smash​/get​/diva2:1348899​/FULLTEXT01​.pdf 23 June, 2020
Bohnacker, U., Lindgren, J., & Öztekin, B.
(2019) Understanding MAIN picture stories: A study of Swedish-Turkish children aged 4–7. Oral presentation at Bilingual Acquisition of Language and Literacy (BiALL) conference, 24 May 2019, Leibniz-ZAS, Berlin.
Bohnacker, U., Öztekin, B., & Lindgren, J.
this volume). Bilingual Turkish-Swedish children’s understanding of MAIN picture sequences: Individual variation, age, language and task effects. In U. Bohnacker & N. Gagarina Eds. Developing narrative comprehension: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives Amsterdam John Benjamins Crossref
Bohn-Gettler, C. M., Rapp, D. N., van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P. & White, M. J.
(2011) Adults’ and children’s monitoring of story events in the service of comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 39(6), 992–1011. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, D. D., Lile, J., & Burns, B. M.
(2011) Basic language skills and young children’s understanding of causal connections during storytelling. Reading Psychology, 32(4), 372–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burris, S. E. & Brown, D. D.
(2014) When all children comprehend: increasing the external validity of narrative comprehension development research. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(168), 1–16.Google Scholar
Curenton, M.
(2010) Understanding the landscapes of stories: The association between preschoolers’ narrative comprehension and production skills and cognitive abilities. Early Child Development and Care, 181(6), 791–808. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Döring, N., & Bortz, J.
(2016) Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation in den Sozial- und Humanwissenschaften. Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fey, M. E., Catts, H. W., Proctor-Williams, K., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X.
(2004) Oral and written story composition skills of children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(6), 1301–1318. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C.
(2011) Listening text comprehension of explicit and implicit information in preschoolers: The role of verbal and inferential skills. Discourse Processes, 48(2), 119–138. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J.
(2012) MAIN: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 56. Berlin: Leibniz Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft.Google Scholar
(2015) Assessment of narrative abilities in bilingual children. In S. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 243–276). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J.
(2019) MAIN: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives – Revised. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 63. Berlin: Leibniz Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft.Google Scholar
Gardner-Neblett, N., & Sideris, J.
(2018) Different tales: The role of gender in the oral narrative – Reading link among African American children. Child Development, 89(4), 1328–1342. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gerson, S. A., & Woodward, A. L.
(2013) The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means-end training. Infancy, 18(2), 289–302. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A., Plunkett, K., & Schafer, G.
(2000) Infant vocabulary development assessed with a British communicative development inventory. Journal of Child Language, 27(3), 689–705. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R.
(2014) Language impairment from 4 to 12 years: Prediction and etiology. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 850–864. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hayward, D. V., Schneider, P., & Gillam, R. B.
(2009) Age and task-related effects on young children’s understanding of a complex picture story. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 55(1), 54–72.Google Scholar
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C.
(1988) Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 53–69. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Isbell, R., Sobol, J., Lindauer, L., & Lowrance, A.
(2004) The effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral language complexity and story comprehension of young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 32(3), 157–163. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
John, S. F., Lui, M., & Tannock, R.
(2003) Children’s story retelling and comprehension using a new narrative resource. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 18(1–2), 91–113. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., Kaderavek, J. N., Ukrainetz, T. A., Eisenberg, S. L., & Gillam, R. B.
(2006) The Index of Narrative Microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(2), 177–191. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kapalková, S., Polisenská, K., Marková, L., & Fenton, J.
(2016) Narrative abilities in early successive bilingual Slovak-English children: A cross-language comparison. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 145–164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karasinski, C., & Ellis Weismer, S.
(2010) Comprehension of inferences in discourse processing by adolescents with and without language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(5), 1268–1279. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kendeou, P., Bohn-Gettler, C., White, M., & van den Broek, P.
(2008) Children’s inference generation across different media. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(3), 259–272. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Y.-S.
(2015) Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development, 86(1), 128–144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2016) Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kunnari, S., Välimaa, T., & Laukkanen-Nevala, P.
(2016) Macrostructure in the narratives of monolingual Finnish and bilingual Finnish–Swedish children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 123–144. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lange, B. P., Euler, H. A., & Zaretsky, E.
(2016) Sex differences in language competence of 3- to 6-year-old children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 1417–1438. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liles, B. Z., Duffy, R. J., Merritt, D. D., & Purcell, S. L.
(1995) Measurement of narrative discourse ability in children with language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(2), 415–425. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lindgren, J.
(2018) Developing narrative competence. Swedish, Swedish-German and Swedish-Turkish children aged 4–6 (Studia Linguistica Upsaliensia 19). Uppsala: Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. S., van den Broek, P., Kremer, K. E., Kendeou, P., White, M., & Lorch, E. P.
(2008) The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29(4), 327–365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P.
(2007) Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children’s on-and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 22(3), 323–340. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2000) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maital, S. L., Dromi, E., Sagi, A. & Bornstein, M. H.
(2000) The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory: Language specific properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 43–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K.
(2018) Individual differences in children’s pragmatic ability: A review of associations with formal language, social cognition, and executive functions. Language Learning and Development, 14(3), 186–223. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Maviş, I., Tunçer, M., & Gagarina, N.
(2016) Macrostructure components in narrations of Turkish–German bilingual children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 69–89. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meisels, S. J., Marsden, D. B., Wiske Stone, M., & Henderson, L. W.
(1997) Examiner’s manual: Early screening inventory-revised. Ann Arbor, MI: Rebus.Google Scholar
Melzer, J., Rißling, J.-K., & Petermann, F.
(2016) Kognitive Kompetenzen und Sprachentwicklung bei Kindern im Alter zwischen vier und fünf Jahren. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 27(1), 37–51. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mickley, M., & Renner, G.
(2010) Intelligenztheorie für die Praxis: Auswahl, Anwendung und Interpretation deutschsprachiger Testverfahren für Kinder und Jugendliche auf Grundlage der CHC-Theorie. Klinische Diagnostik und Evaluation, 3(4), 447–466.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J., Poluenko, A., & Marinellie, S. A.
(2009) Role of working memory in children’s understanding spoken narrative: A preliminary investigation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(3), 485–509. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Murfett, R., Powell, M. B., & Snow, P. C.
(2008) The effect of intellectual disability on the adherence of child witnesses to a “story grammar” framework. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 33(1), 2–11. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.
(2010) Developmental narratives of the experiencing child. Child Development Perspectives, 4(1), 42–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A.
(2008) The elementary forms of narrative coherence in young children’s storytelling. Narrative Inquiry, 18(2), 299–325.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., Brockmeyer, C., de Sá, A., & Ilgaz, H.
(2014) Narrative performance, peer group culture, and narrative development in a preschool classroom. In A. Cekaite, S. Blum-Kulka, V. Grover, & E. Teubal (Eds.), Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other (pp. 42–62). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Orrantia, J., Múñez, D., & Tarín, J.
(2014) Connecting goals and actions during reading: the role of illustrations. Reading and Writing, 27(1), 153–170. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Otwinowska, A., Mieszkowska, K., Białecka-Pikul, M., Opacki, M., & Haman, E.
(2018) Retelling a model story improves the narratives of Polish-English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 1–25. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pavlenko, A.
(2008) Narrative analysis. In L. Wei & M. G. Moyer (Eds.), The Blackwell guide to research methods in bilingualism and multilingualism (pp. 311–325). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A.
(1991) Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York, NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Quasthoff, U., & Stude, J.
(2018) Narrative Interaktion: Entwicklungsaufgabe und Ressource des Erzählerwerbs. Zeitschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik, 48(2), 249–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C., Bulheller, J., & Häcker, J. H.
(2002) Coloured Progressive Matrices (CPM). Frankfurt: Pearson.Google Scholar
Renner, G., & Mickley, M.
(2015) Intelligenzdiagnostik im Vorschulalter: CHC-theoretisch fundierte Untersuchungsplanung und Cross-battery-assessment. Frühförderung interdisziplinär, 34(2), 67–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ringmann, S.
(2014) Therapie der Makrostruktur von Erzählungen. Sprachförderung und Sprachtherapie in Schule und Praxis, 3(3), 147–155.Google Scholar
(2013) Therapie der Erzählfähigkeit. In S. Ringmann & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Handbuch Spracherwerb und Sprachentwicklungsstörungen. Schuleingangsphase (pp. 163–187). Munich: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Roch, M., Florit, E., & Levorato, C.
(2016) Narrative competence of Italian-English bilingual children between 5 and 7 years. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 49–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Creamer, S.
(2010) Assessment of strategic processing during narrative comprehension in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(4), 661–671. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schöler, H., & Brunner, M.
(2008) Heidelberger auditives Screening in der Einschulungsuntersuchung (HASE). Binswangen: Westra.Google Scholar
Sommerville, J. A., & Woodward, A. L.
(2005) Infants’ sensitivity to the causal features of means–end support sequences in action and perception. Infancy, 8(2), 119–145. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G.
(1979) An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), Discourse processing: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Szagun, G.
(2006) Sprachentwicklung beim Kind. Weinheim: Beltz.Google Scholar
Szaflarski, J. P., Altaye, M., Rajagopal, A., Eaton, K., Meng, X., Plante, E. & Holland, S. K.
(2012) A 10-year longitudinal fMRI study of narrative comprehension in children and adolescents. Neuroimage, 63(3), 1188–1195. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M.
(1992) The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes, 15(3), 249–275. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P., & Liu, L.
(1988) A model for generating questions that assess and promote comprehension. Questioning Exchange, 2(1), 25–38.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., Peristeri, E., & Andreou, M.
(2016) Narrative production in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 195–216. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, M. T., Miranda, R. A., & Travers, M.
(2008) Sex differences in the neurocognition of language. In J. B. Becker, K. J. Berkley, N. Geary, E. Hampson, J. Herman, & E. Young (Eds.), Sex on the brain: From genes to behavior (pp. 291–309). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Lynch, J. S., Naslund, J., Ievers-Landis, C. E., & Verduin, K.
(2003) The development of comprehension of main ideas in narratives: Evidence from the selection of titles. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 707–718. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Kleeck, A., Lange, A., & Schwarz, A. L.
(2011) The effect of race and maternal education level on children’s retells of the Renfrew Bus Story-North American Edition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(6), 1546–1561. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L.
(1962) Thought and word. In L. Vygotsky, E. Hanfmann, & G. Vakar (Eds.), Thought and language (pp. 119–153). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wallentin, M.
(2009) Putative sex differences in verbal abilities and language cortex: A critical review. Brain & Language, 108(3), 175–183. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Westerveld, M. F., Gillon, G. & Boyd, L.
(2012) Evaluating the clinical utility of the Profile of Oral Narrative Ability for 4-year-old children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14(2), 130–140. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Westby, C. E.
(2005) Assessing and facilitating text comprehension problems. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), Language and reading disabilities (pp. 157–232). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Ralli, Asimina M., Elena Kazali, Maria Kanellou, Angeliki Mouzaki, Fotini Antoniou, Vasiliki Diamanti & Sofia Papaioannou
2021. Oral Language and Story Retelling During Preschool and Primary School Years: Developmental Patterns and Interrelationships. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.