References
Aissen, J.
(2003) Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 21, 435–483. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alarcón, I.
(2009) The processing of gender agreement in L1 and L2 Spanish: Evidence from reaction time data. Hispania, 92(4), 814–828.Google Scholar
(2011) Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 332–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amaral, L., & Roeper, T.
(2014) Multiple grammars and second language representation. Second Language Research, 30, 3–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anglin, J. M.
(1993) Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(10, Serial No. 238). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arunachalam, S., & He, A. X.
(2018) Children’s acquisition of nouns that denote events. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 29–44). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A.
(2007) Developing linguistic knowledge and language use during adolescence. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 347–367). Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, J.
(1993) Topics in the syntax of nominal structures across Romance (Unpublished PhD dissertation). City University of New York.Google Scholar
Bosque, I., & Picallo, C.
(1996) Postnominal adjectives in Spanish DPs. Journal of Linguistics, 32, 349–385. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bruhn de Garavito, J., & White, L.
(2002) The second language acquisition of Spanish DPs: The status of grammatical features. In A. T. Pérez-Leroux & J. Liceras (Eds.), The acquisition of Spanish morphosyntax: The L1/L2 connection (pp. 153–178). Kluwer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bylund, E.
(2009) Maturational constraints and first language attrition. Language Learning, 59, 687–715. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstens, V.
(1991) The morphology and syntax of determiner phrases in Kiswahili (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of California.Google Scholar
Cinque, G.
(1994) On the evidence for partial N-movement in the Romance DP. In G. Cinque, J. Koster, J. Y. Pollock, L. Rizzi, & R. Zanuttini (Eds.), Paths towards Universal Grammar: Studies in Honor of Richard S. Kayne (pp. 85–110). Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
(2010) The syntax of adjectives. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A.
(2013) Cross-linguistic influence at the syntax proper: Interrogative subject-verb inversion in heritage Spanish. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 71–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) The status of interrogative subject-verb inversion in Spanish–English bilingual children. Lingua, 180, 124–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Frank, J.
(2014) On the role of experience and age-related effects: Evidence from the Spanish CP. Second Language Research, 31, 3–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., Miller, L., Pasquarella, A., & Chen, X.
(2017) The role of literacy instruction in the development of Spanish as a heritage language during childhood. The Heritage Language Journal, 14(2), 100–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Perez-Tattam, R.
(2016) Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature re-assembly approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 332–350.Google Scholar
Cuza, A., Pérez-Tattam, R., Barajas, E., Miller, L., & Sadowski, C.
(2013) The development of tense and aspect morphology in child and adult heritage speakers. In J. W. Schwieter (Ed.), Innovative research and practices in second language acquisition and bilingualism (pp. 193–219). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., Reyes, N., & Lustres, E.
(2021) The production of ser and estar in child and adult heritage speakers of Spanish. Lingua, 249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daskalaki, E., Blom, E., Chondrogianni, V., & Paradis, J.
(2020) Effects of parental input quality in child heritage language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 47(4), 709–736. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demonte, V.
(1999) El adjetivo: Classes y usos. La posición del adjetivo en el sintagma nominal. In I. Bosque & V. Demonte (Eds.), Gramática descriptiva de la lengua español (pp.129–215). Espasa Calpe.Google Scholar
Domínguez, L., Hicks, G., & Slabakova, R.
(2019) Terminology choice in generative acquisition research: The case of “incomplete acquisition” in heritage language grammars. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41(2), 241–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Flores, C.
(2014) Losing a language in childhood: A longitudinal case study on language attrition. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 562–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Franceschina F.
(2001) Morphological or syntactic deficits in near-native speakers? An assessment of some current proposalsSecond Language Research 2001;17(3):213–247.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C.
(2002) Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In K. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 207–219). De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giancaspro, D. J.
(2017) Heritage speakers’ production and comprehension of lexically-and contextually-selected subjunctive mood morphology (Unpublished PhD dissertation). Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Giancaspro D.
& Sánchez L. (2021) Me, mi, my: Innovation and variability in heritage speakers’ knowledge of inalienable possession. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics, 6(1): 31. 1–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A.
(2012) Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gürel, A.
(2004) Selectivity in L2-induced L1 attrition: A psycholinguistic account. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 53–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, J.
(1991) The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22, 65–88.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., & Core, C.
(2015) Input and language development in bilingually developing children. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(2), 215–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hulk, A., & Müller, N.
(2000) Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 3, 227–244. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hur, E.
(2020) Verbal lexical frequency and DOM in heritage speakers of Spanish. In A. Mardale & S. Montrul (Eds.), The acquisition of differential object marking (pp. 207–235). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hur, E., Lopez Otero, J. C., & Sanchez, L.
(2020) Gender agreement and assignment in Spanish heritage speakers: Does frequency matter? Languages, 5, 48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jia, R., & Paradis, J.
(2015) The use of referring expressions in narratives by Mandarin heritage language children and the role of language environment factors in predicting individual differences. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 737–752. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P., Conboy, B., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J.
(2005) Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the “critical period”. Language Learning and Development, 1, 237–264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., & Rothman, J.
(2018) Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(5), 564–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lamarche, J.
(1991) Problems for N°-movement to Num-P. Probus, 3(2): 215–236. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D.
(1998) Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14(4), 359–375. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Feature-assembly in second language acquisition. In J. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 106–140). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(2009) Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 25(2), 173–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leonetti, M.
(2008) Specificity in clitic doubling and in differential object marking. Probus, 20, 35–69. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Fernández, A., Gallo, P., & Mariscal, S.
(1994) La adquisición de la lengua española. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
López-Otero, Julio Cesar
2019On the acceptability of the Spanish DOM among Romanian-Spanish bilinguals, New Brunswick: Rutgers University, unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
López-Otero, J. C.
(2019) On the acceptability of the Spanish DOM among Romanian-Spanish bilinguals (Unpublished manuscript). Rutgers University,Google Scholar
Mardale, A.
(2008) Microvariation within differential object marking: Data from Romance. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique, LIII(4), 449–467.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S.
(2009) Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: starting small. Journal of Child Language 36: 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitrofanova, N., Rodina, Y., Urek, O., & Westergaard, M.
(2018) Bilinguals’ sensitivity to grammatical gender cues in Russian: The role of cumulative input, proficiency, and dominance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 18–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S.
(2002) Incomplete acquisition and attrition of Spanish tense/aspect distinctions in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 39–68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2004) Subject and object expression in Spanish heritage speakers: A case of morphosyntactic convergence. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7(2), 125–142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Incomplete acquisition of tense-aspect and mood in Spanish heritage speakers. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 13(3), 239–269. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016) Heritage language acquisition. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Davidson, J., De La Fuente, I., & Foote, R.
(2014) Early language experience facilitates the processing of gender agreement in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 118–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., de la Fuente, I., Davidson, J., & Foote, R.
(2013) The role of experience in the acquisition and production of diminutives and gender in Spanish: Evidence from L2 learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research, 29, 87–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñan, S.
(2008) Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58(3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K.
(2007) Command of gender agreement in school age Spanish English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(3), 301–330. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Sánchez-Walker, N.
(2013) Differential object marking in child and adult Spanish heritage speakers. Language Acquisition, 20(2), 109–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E.
(2002) Language and literacy in bilingual children. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Otheguy, R.
(2016) The linguistic competence of second-generation bilinguals: A critique of incomplete acquisition. In C. Tortora, M. den Dikken, I. L. Montoya, & T. O’Neill (Eds.), Romance linguistics 2013 (pp. 301–320). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pascual y Cabo, D., & Rothman, J.
(2012) The (il)logical problem of heritage speaker bilingualism and incomplete acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 33, 450–455. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Cortes, S., Putnam, M., & Sánchez, L.
(2019) Differential access: Asymmetries in accessing features and building representations in heritage language grammars. Languages, 4(4), 81–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Cuza, A., & Thomas, D.
(2011) From parental attitudes to input condition Spanish-English bilingual development in Toronto. In K. Potowski & J. Rothman (Eds.), Bilingual youth: Spanish in English-speaking societies, (pp. 49–176). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M.
(1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18(3), 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M.
(2006) Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14, 191–262.Google Scholar
(2008) Gender under incomplete acquisition: Heritage speakers’ knowledge of noun categorization. Heritage Language Journal, 6(1), 40–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Reanalysis in adult heritage language: A case for attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 3(4), 305–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G.
(2019) Understanding heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 4–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2020) A road map for heritage language research. BLC, 23(1), 50–55.Google Scholar
(2020) A road map for heritage language research. BLC, 23(1), 50–55.Google Scholar
Putnam, M., & Sánchez, L.
(2013) What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition? Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(4), 478–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, M.
(2008) The acquisition of differential object marking in Spanish. Probus, 20(1), 111–145. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J.
(2007) Heritage speaker competence differences, language change, and input type: Inflected infinitives in heritage Brazilian Portuguese. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(4), 359–389. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J.
(2011) Proficiency and animacy effects on L2 gender agreement processes during comprehension. Language Learning, 61(1), 80–116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, L.
(1996) Syntactic structures in nominals: A comparative study of southern Quechua and Spanish (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Southern California.Google Scholar
Sánchez, L.
(2019) Bilingual alignments. Languages, 4(82), 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schmid, M. S., Köpke, B.
(2007) Bilingualism and attrition. In Language attrition: Theoretical perspectives, Köpke, B., Schmid, M. S., Keijzer, M., Dostert, S. (eds.), pp. 1–8. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., & Fuchs, Z.
(2018) In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sera, M.D.
(1992) To be or not to be: use and acquisition of the Spanish copula. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 408–427.Google Scholar
Serratrice, L., Sorace, A., & Paoli, S.
(2004) Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English-Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 7, 183–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(1991) Spanish language attrition in a contact situation with English. In H. Seliger & R. Vago (Eds.), First language attrition (pp.151–171). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018a) Simultaneous bilingualism: Early developments, incomplete later outcomes? International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 497–512. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018b) Bilingual acquisition: Difference or incompleteness? In N. Lapidus Shin & D. Erker (Eds.), Questioning theoretical primitives in linguistic inquiry: Papers in honor of Ricardo Otheguy (pp. 245–268). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C., & Montanari, S.
(2008) The acquisition of ser, estar (and be) by a Spanish–English bilingual child: The early stages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(3), 341–360. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C.
(2014) Bilingual Language Acquisition: Spanish and English in the First Six Years. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sorace, A.
(2000) Differential effects of attrition in the L1 syntax of near-native L2 speakers. In C. Howell, S. Fish, & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds), Proceedings of the 24th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.719–725). Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
(2005) Selective optionality in language development. In L. Cornips & K. Corrigan (Eds.), Syntax and variation: reconciling the biological and the social (pp. 55–80). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011) Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 1–33. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Syrett, K., Lingwall, A., Pérez-Cortes, S., Austin, J., Sánchez, L., Baker, H., Germak, C., & Arias-Amaya, A.
(2017) Differences between Spanish monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children in their calculation of entailment-based scalar implicatures. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2(1): 31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teschner, R., & Russell, W.
(1984) The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An invers dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics, 1, 115–132.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E.
(2017) Amount trumps timing in bilingual vocabulary acquisition: Effects of input in simultaneous and sequential school-age bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23, 236–255. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ticio, E., & Avram, L.
(2015) The acquisition of differential object marking in Spanish and Romanian: Semantic scales or semantic features? Revue Roumaine de Linguistique, 60(4), 383–401.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F.
(2004) First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 257–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S.
(2016) Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In E. Nicoladis & S. Montanari (Eds.), Lifespan perspectives on bilingualism (pp. 136–196). De Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valdes, G.
(2001) Heritage language students: Profiles and possibilities. In J. K. Peyton, D. A. Ranard, & S. McGinnis (Eds), Heritage languages in America: Preserving a national resource (pp. 37–77). Center for Applied Linguistics & Delta Systems Company.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, E., Faure, A., Ramírez-Trujillo, A. P., Barski, E., Pangtay, Y., & Díez, A.
(2012) Gender and heritage Spanish bilingual grammars: A study of code-mixed determiner phrases and copula constructions. Hispania, 95(3), 481–494.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-MacGregor, M., & Leung, Y. K.
(2004) Gender and number agreement in nonnative Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zagona, K.
(2002) The syntax of Spanish. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar