Part of
Understanding Language and Cognition through Bilingualism: In honor of Ellen Bialystok
Edited by Gigi Luk, John A.E. Anderson and John G. Grundy
[Studies in Bilingualism 64] 2023
► pp. 122141
References
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S. F., & Costa, A.
(2012) Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2076–2086. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn, R. L., & Syme, S. L.
(1994) Socioeconomic status and health. The challenge of the gradient. The American Psychologist, 49(1), 15–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. A., & Rovee-Collier, C.
(1991) Contextual variation and memory retrieval at six months. Child Development, 62(5), 1155–1165. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., Davidson, D. J., & Carreiras, M.
(2014) Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antovich, D. M., & Graf Estes, K.
(2018) Learning across languages: Bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12548. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E.
(2016) Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development, 87(4), 1277–1290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barker, E. D., Kirkham, N., Ng, J., & Jensen, S. K. G.
(2013) Prenatal maternal depression symptoms and nutrition, and child cognitive function. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 203(6), 417–421. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, R., & Brito, N.
(2013) From specificity to flexibility: Developmental changes during infancy. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook on the development of children’s memory (Ch. 20). Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barr, R., Marrott, H., & Rovee-Collier, C.
(2003) The role of sensory preconditioning in memory retrieval by preverbal infants. Learning & Behavior, 31(2), 111–123. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1992) Selective attention in cognitive processing: The bilingual edge. In R. J. Harris (Ed.), Advances in psychology (Vol. 83, pp. 501–513). North-Holland. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009) Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12(4), 499–501. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010) Bilingualism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 559–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 117–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S.
(2011) Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. [URL]
Borovsky, D., & Rovee-Collier, C.
(1990) Contextual constraints on memory retrieval at six months. Child Development, 61(5), 1569–1583. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brito, N., & Barr, R.
(2012) Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental Science, 15(6), 812–816. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Flexible memory retrieval in bilingual 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(5), 1165–1163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brito, N. H., Grenell, A., & Barr, R.
(2014) Specificity of the bilingual advantage for memory: Examining cued recall, generalization, and working memory in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual toddlers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brito, N. H., & Noble, K. G.
(2014) Socioeconomic status and structural brain development. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2018) The independent and interacting effects of socioeconomic status and dual-language use on brain structure and cognition. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12688. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brito, N. H., Sebastian-Galles, N., & Barr, R.
(2015) Differences in language exposure and its effects on memory flexibility in monolingual, bilingual, and trilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 670–682. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
(2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
(2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Christofides, L. N., & Swidinsky, R.
(2010) The economic returns to the knowledge and use of a second official language: English in Quebec and French in the rest-of-Canada. Canadian Public Policy. Analyse de Politiques, 36(2), 137–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clearfield, M. W., & Niman, L. C.
(2012) SES affects infant cognitive flexibility. Infant Behavior & Development, 35(1), 29–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Combs-Orme, T., & Cain, D. S.
(2006) Poverty and the daily lives of infants. Journal of Children and Poverty, 12(1), 1–20). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Comishen, K. J., Bialystok, E., & Adler, S. A.
(2019) The impact of bilingual environments on selective attention in infancy. Developmental Science, 22(4), e12797. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J.
(1976) The influence of bilingualism on cognitive growth: A synthesis of research findings and explanatory hypotheses. Working Papers on Bilingualism, 9, 1–43.Google Scholar
(1979) Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 222–251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Angiulli, A., Herdman, A., Stapells, D., & Hertzman, C.
(2008) Children’s event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology, 22(3), 293–300. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
De Cat, C., Gusnanto, A., & Serratrice, L.
(2018) Identifying a threshold for the executive function advantage in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(1), 119–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Della Rosa, P. A., Videsott, G., Borsa, V. M., Canini, M., Weekes, B. S., Franceschini, R., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) A neural interactive location for multilingual talent. Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 49(2), 605–608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A.
(2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duncan, G. J., & Magnuson, K. A.
(2003) Off with Hollingshead: Socioeconomic resources, parenting, and child development. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 83–106). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H.
(1997) Declarative memory: Insights from cognitive neurobiology. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 547–572. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2000) A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 1(1), 41–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C. J., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E.
(2012) Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1364–1371. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A.
(2013) SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garcia-Sierra, A., Ramírez-Esparza, N., & Kuhl, P. K.
(2016) Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. International Journal of Psychophysiology: Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 110, 1–17. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas-Guasch, N., Roberts, E. J., Hughes, E. K., & Jones, L.
(2014) Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. [URL]. DOI logo
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F.
(2013) Prosody cues word order in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Nature Communications, 4, 1490. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L.
(1999) Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Warren, S. F., Montgomery, J. K., Greenwood, C. R., Kimbrough Oller, D., Hansen, J. H. L., & Paul, T. D.
(2017) Mapping the early language environment using all-day recordings and automated analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology / American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 26(2), 248–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ginsborg, J.
(2006) The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and use. In J. Clegg & J. Ginsborg (Eds.), Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice (pp. 9–27). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Goriot, C., Broersma, M., McQueen, J. M., Unsworth, S., & van Hout, R.
(2018) Language balance and switching ability in children acquiring English as a second language. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 168–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greco, C., Hayne, H., & Rovee-Collier, C.
(1990) Roles of function, reminding, and variability in categorization by 3-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 617–633. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J.
(2009) Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 65–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halle, T., Forry, N., Hair, E., Perper, K., Wandner, L., Wessel, J., & Vick, J.
(2009) Disparities in early learning and development: Lessons from the early childhood longitudinal Study--Birth Cohort (ECLS-B). Child Trends. Retrieved on 6 December 2022 from [URL]
Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H.
(2019) Bilingualism narrows socioeconomic disparities in executive functions and self-regulatory behaviors during early childhood: Evidence From the early childhood longitudinal study. Child Development, 90(4), pp. 1215–1235. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayne, H.
(2006) Age-related changes in infant memory retrieval: Implications for knowledge acquisition. Processes of Brain and Cognitive Development. Attention and Performance, XXI, 209–231.Google Scholar
Hayne, H., MacDonald, S., & Barr, R.
(1997) Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 20(2), 233–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herbert, J., Gross, J., & Hayne, H.
(2007) Crawling is associated with more flexible memory retrieval by 9-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 10(2), 183–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Herbert, J., & Hayne, H.
(2000) Memory retrieval by 18–30-month-olds: Age-related changes in representational flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 473–484. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E.
(2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review: DR, 26(1), 55–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
(1998) The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(4), 603–629. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hornickel, J., Lin, D., & Kraus, N.
(2013) Speech-evoked auditory brainstem responses reflect familial and cognitive influences. Developmental Science, 16(1), 101–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V.
(2010) Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. J. H., & Herbert, J. S.
(2006) Using deferred imitation to understand the process of change in infant memory development. Infant and Child Development, 15(2), 215–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J.
(2009) Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325(5940), 611–612. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N.
(2016) Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Developmental Science, 19(6), 881–891. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Marian, V., & Kraus, N.
(2014) Bilingualism increases neural response consistency and attentional control: Evidence for sensory and cognitive coupling. Brain and Language, 128(1), 34–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuchirko, Y.
(2019) On differences and deficits: A critique of the theoretical and methodological underpinnings of the word gap. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 19(4), 533–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Learmonth, A. E., Lamberth, R., & Rovee-Collier, C.
(2004) Generalization of deferred imitation during the first year of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88(4), 297–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, Y., & Waite, L. J.
(2005) The impact of childhood and adult SES on physical, mental, and cognitive well-being in later life. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(2), S93–S101. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marcus, G. F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., & Vishton, P. M.
(1999) Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283(5398), 77–80. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mezzacappa, E.
(2004) Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Morton, J. B., Bruce Morton, J., & Harper, S. N.
(2007) What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10(6), 719–726. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & Webb, S. J.
(2003) A cognitive neuroscience perspective on early memory development. In M. de Haan & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of development (pp. 99–125). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Noble, K. G., Engelhardt, L. E., Brito, N. H., Mack, L. J., Nail, E. J., Angal, J., Barr, R., Fifer, W. P., Elliott, A. J., & PASS Network
(2015) Socioeconomic disparities in neurocognitive development in the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(5), 535–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., Akshoomoff, N., Amaral, D. G., Bloss, C. S., Libiger, O., Schork, N. J., Murray, S. S., Casey, B. J., Chang, L., Ernst, T. M., Frazier, J. A., Gruen, J. R., Kennedy, D. N., Van Zijl, P., … Sowell, E. R.
(2015) Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 773–778. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Thompson, B. L., Gharib, A., Schlueter, L., Reilly, E., Valdes, V., Roberts, S., Conroy, K., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A.
(2019) Association of perceived maternal stress during the perinatal period with electroencephalography patterns in 2-month-old infants. JAMA Pediatrics, 173(6), 561–570. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E.
(2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82(6), 1834–1849. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Plude, D. J., Enns, J. T., & Brodeur, D.
(1994) The development of selective attention: A life-span overview. Acta Psychologica, 86(2–3), 227–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G.
(2012) Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(4), 535–551. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Proctor, B. D., Semega, J. L., & Kollar, M. A.
(2016) Income and poverty in the United States: 2015. US Census Bureau, Current Population Reports, P60–P256. Retrieved on 6 December 2022 from [URL]
Richards, J. E.
(1998) Development of selective attention in young infants: Enhancement and attenuation of startle reflex by attention. Developmental Science, 1(1), 45–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Richmond, J., & Nelson, C. A.
(2007) Accounting for change in declarative memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review: DR, 27(3), 349–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., & Cuevas, K.
(2008) The development of infant memory. In M. L. Courage & N. Cowan (Eds.), The development of memory in infancy and childhood (pp. 23–54). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L.
(2008) Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185–205. [URL]. DOI logo
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
(2009) Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951–953. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Skoe, E., Krizman, J., & Kraus, N.
(2013) The impoverished brain: Disparities in maternal education affect the neural response to sound. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(44), 17221–17231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. B., Colunga, E., & Yoshida, H.
(2010) Knowledge as process: Contextually-cued attention and early word learning. Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1287–1314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Tessner, K. D., & Toga, A. W.
(2001) Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 21(22), 8819–8829. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Spear, N. E.
(1984) The future study of learning and memory from a psychobiological perspective. In V. Sarris & A. Parducci (Eds.), Perspectives in psychological experimentation (pp. 87–103). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sperry, D. E., Sperry, L. L., & Miller, P. J.
(2019) Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child development, 90(4), 1303–1318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Statistics, U. S. D. of H. &. H. S. N. C. F. H., US Department of Health & Human Services; National Center for Health Statistics, & Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics
(2001) America’s children: Key national indicators of well-being 2001. PsycEXTRA Dataset. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M.
(1973) Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B.
(2011) Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 412–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar