Part of
Understanding Language and Cognition through Bilingualism: In honor of Ellen Bialystok
Edited by Gigi Luk, John A.E. Anderson and John G. Grundy
[Studies in Bilingualism 64] 2023
► pp. 318342
References
Abutalebi, J., Guidi, L., Borsa, V., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., Parris, B. A., & Weekes, B. S.
(2015) Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychologia, 69, 201–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S.
(2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ansaldo, A. I., Ghazi-Saidi, L., & Adrover-Roig, D.
(2015) Interference control in elderly bilinguals: Appearances can be misleading. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 37, 455–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antón, E., Fernández García, Y., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A.
(2016) Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J.
(2014) Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959–963. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M.
(2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
(2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 34, 828–828. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J.
(2006) Executive control in a modified anti-saccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1341–1354.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M.
(2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
(2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 696–705. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braver, T. S.
(2012) The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual-mechanisms framework. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 106–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braver, T. S. & West, R.
(2008) Working memory, executive control, and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 311–372). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E.
(2002) Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B.
(1971) Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez De Sather, J. C.
(2001) Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37, 715–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chertkow, H., Whitehead, V., Phillips, N., Wolfson, C., Atherton, J., & Bergman, H.
(2010) Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer’s disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 24, 118–125. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B.
(2008) How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 34, 302–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Connelly, S. L., Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T.
(1991) Age and reading: The impact of distraction. Psychology and Aging, 6, 533–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, A., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N.
(1995) Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M.
(1983) On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 302, 341–359. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1986) A functional account of age differences in memory. In F. Klix & H. Hagendorf (Eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities. North-Holland.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E.
(2006) Cognition through the lifespan: Mechanisms of change. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 131–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2008) Lifespan cognitive development: The roles of representation and control. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 557–601). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., Gillingham, S. & Stuss, D. T.
(2018) Alpha Span: A measure of working memory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 141–152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & McDowd, J. M.
(1987) Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 474–479.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., Morris, L. W., Morris, R. G., & Loewen, E. R.
(1990) Relations between source amnesia and frontal lobe functioning in a normal elderly sample. Psychology and Aging, 5, 148–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M., & Rose, N. S.
(2012) Training cognition: Parallels with physical fitness? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 51–52. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Tardif, T.
(1987) Working memory and reading skill re-examined. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance XII (pp. 491–508). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Dash, T., Berroir, P., Joanette, Y., & Ansaldo, A. I.
(2019) Alerting, orienting, and executive control: The effect of bilingualism and age on the subcomponents of attention. Frontiers in Neurology, 10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
D’Esposito, M., & Postle, B. R.
(2015) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 115–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fernandes, M. A., Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., & Kreuger, S.
(2007) Effects of bilingualism, aging, and semantic relatedness on memory under divided attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 128–141. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D.
(2013) Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), 387–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, E.
(1985) Akinesia, tardive dysmentia, and frontal lobe disorder in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 11, 255–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R. I., & Jernigan, T. L.
(2007) The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 197–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Werner, G.
(2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goral, M., Campanelli, L., & Spiro, A.
(2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 79–89. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grady, C., Luk, G., Craik, F., Bialystok, E.
(2015) Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia, 66, 170–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J.
(2013) Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grundy, J. G., Anderson, J. A., & Bialystok, E.
(2017) Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1396(1), 183–201. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grundy, J. G., & Timmer, K.
(2017) Bilingualism and working memory capacity: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Second Language Research, 33(3), 325–340. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A. & Douglas, R. M.
(1985) Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research, 58, 455–472. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hallet, P. E. & Adams, B. D.
(1980) The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel, voluntary occulomotor task. Vision Research, 20, 329–339. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L.
(2015) Inhibitory deficit hypothesis. In S. K. Whitbourne (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging. Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T.
(1979) Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hasher, L. & Zacks, R. T.
(1988) Working memory, comprehension and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 193–225). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hay, J. F., & Jacoby, L. L.
(1999) Separating habit and recollection in young and elderly adults: Effects of elaborative processing and distinctiveness. Psychology and Aging, 14, 122–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heaton, R. K.
(1981) A manual for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A. B., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2010) The impact of bilingualism on the executive control and orienting networks of attention. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 315–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hernández, M., Costa, A., & Humphreys, G. W.
(2012) Escaping capture: Bilingualism modulates distraction from working memory. Cognition, 122, 37–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M.
(2011) Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18(4), 625–658. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L.
(1982) The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in relation to concepts of cognitive psychology and aging in adulthood. In F. I. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 237–278). Plenum Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ihle, A., Oris, M., Fagot, D., & Kliegel, M.
(2016) The relation of the number of languages spoken to performance in different cognitive abilities in old age. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38, 1103–1114. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Incera, S., & McLennan, C. T.
(2018) Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 25, 443–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, L. L.
(1991) A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jennings, J. M., & Jacoby, L. L.
(1993) Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging, attention, and control. Psychology and Aging, 8, 283–293. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W.
(2004) The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 189–217. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S.
(1983) Boston naming test. Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Kavé, G., Eyal, N., Shorek, A., & Cohen-Mansfield, J.
(2008) Multilingualism and cognitive state in the oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 23, 70–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V.
(2014) No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 640–648. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A.
(2012) Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 356–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) A behavioural and electrophysiological investigation of the effect of bilingualism on aging and cognitive control. Neuropsychologia, 94, 23–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., & Taler, V.
(2014) Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., Misra, M., & Guo, T.
(2008) Language selection in bilingual speech. Acta Psychologica, 128, 416–430. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lhermitte, F.
(1986) Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. 2. Patient behavior in complex and social situations – the environmental dependency syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 19, 335–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ljungberg, J. K., Hansson, P., Andres, P., Josefsson, M., & Nilsson, L.-G.
(2013) A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PLoS ONE, 8, 1–8. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74, 347–357. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L.
(2011) Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16808–16813. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E.
(2013) Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28, 28–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, M. A., Glisky, E. L., Rubin, S. R., Guynn, M. J., & Routhieaux, B. C.
(1999) Prospective memory: A neuropsychological study. Neuropsychology, 13, 103–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M.-M.
(2002) The human frontal lobes: Transcending the default mode through contingent encoding. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 8–30). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Milner, B.
(1964) Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In J. Warren & K. Akert (Eds.), The frontal granular cortex and behavior (pp. 313–331). McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
(1971) Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G.
(1992) The neuropsychology of memory and aging. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 315–372). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Oberauer, K., Süß, H.-M., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Wittman, W. W.
(2000) Working memory capacity – Facets of a cognitive ability construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1017–1045. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olsen, R. K., Pangelinan, M. M., Bogulski, C., Chakravarty, M. M., Luk, G., Grady, C. L., & Bialystok, E.
(2015) The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Research, 1612, 128–139. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I.
(2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Petrides, M. & Milner, B.
(1982) Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20, 249–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., Moschopoulou, E. & Saddy, J. D.
(2015) The effect of bilingualism on the white matter structure of the brain. PNAS, 112, 1334–1337. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raz, N.
(2000) Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–90). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J.
(1997) ‘Oops!’: Performance correlates of everyday attention failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35, 747–758. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M.
(2010) The effects of bilingualism and age on inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15, 26–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schroeder, S. R., & Marian, V.
(2012) A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 591–601. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Laine, M., Hämäläinen, H., & Hugdahl, K.
(2010) Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 371–378. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soveri, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Laine, M.
(2011) Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 183. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y.
(2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P.
(2007) Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 362, 901–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stuss, D. T. & Craik, F. I. M.
(2020) Alterations in executive functions with aging. In K. M. Heilman & S. Nadeau (Eds.), Cognitive changes in the aging brain. (pp.168–187). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stuss, D. T., Craik, F. I. M., Sayer, L., Franchi, D. & Alexander, M. P.
(1996) Comparison of older people and patients with frontal lobe lesions: Evidence from word list learning. Psychology and Aging, 11, 387–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tao, L., Wang, G., Zhu, M., & Cai, Q.
(2021) Bilingualism and domain-general cognitive functions from a neural perspective: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 264–295. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Mishler, A., Corbett, R., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Trueswell, J. C., & Novick, J. M.
(2016) The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition, 150, 213–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L.
(1992) Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training - A meta-analytic study. Psychology and Aging, 7 (2), 242-251. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W. & Gollan, T. H.
(2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 958–974. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
West, R. L.
(1996) An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–292. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wodniecka, Z., Craik, F. I. M., Luo, L., & Bialystok, E.
(2010) Does bilingualism help memory? Competing effects of verbal ability and executive control. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 575–595. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. M., & Booth, L.
(2004) Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychologica, 115, 167–83. DOI logoGoogle Scholar