Part of
Multifaceted Multilingualism
Edited by Kleanthes K. Grohmann
[Studies in Bilingualism 66] 2024
► pp. 292326
References (130)
References
Abu-Akel, A., Bailey, A. L., & Thum, Y.-M. (2004). Describing the acquisition of determiners in English: A growth modeling approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 33(5), 407–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Afonso, O., Domínguez, A., Álvarez, C., & Morales, D. (2014). Sublexical and lexico-syntactic factors in gender access in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 13–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alarcón, I. (2011). Spanish gender agreement under complete and incomplete acquisition: Early and late bilinguals’ linguistic behavior within the noun phrase. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 332–350. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Chakrani, B. (2013). Gender and number agreement in the oral production of Arabic Heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 16(1), 1–18. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Saadah, E. (2011). Grammatical features of Egyptian and Palestinian heritage speakers’ oral production. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 273–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J., Fiorentino, R., & Gabriele, A. (2012). The processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: An event-related potential investigation of the effects of structural distance. Brain Research, 1456, 49–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Alemán Bañón, J., & Rothman, J. (2016). The role of morphological markedness in the processing of number and gender agreement in Spanish: An event-related potential investigation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(10), 1273–1298. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (1999). Loss of gender agreement in L1 attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23(4), 389–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Antón-Méndez, M. I. (1999). Gender and number: Agreement processing in Spanish (Unpublished PhD dissertation). University of Arizona.Google Scholar
Antón-Méndez, M. I., Nicol, J. L., & Garrett, M. F. (2002). The relation between gender and number agreement processing. Syntax, 5, 1–25. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Au T.-K., Oh, J.-S., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S.-A., & Romo, L. F. (2008). Salvaging a childhood language. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(4), 998–1011. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, M. C. (2003). Lexical categories. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barber, H., & Carreiras, M. (2003). Integrating gender and number information in Spanish word pairs: An ERP study. Cortex, 39(3), 465–482. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2005). Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 137–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barber, H., Salillas, E., & Carreiras, M. (2004). Gender or genders agreement? In M. Carreiras & C. Clifton (Eds.), On-line study of sentence comprehension: Eye-tracking, ERP and beyond (pp. 309–328). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bassano, D., Maillochon, I., & Mottet, S. (2008). Noun grammaticalization and determiner use in French children’s speech: A gradual development with prosodic and lexical influences. Journal of Child Language, 35(2), 403–438. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Bruni, M., Bajo, M. T., & Dussias, P. (2020). Brain potentials reveal differential processing of masculine and feminine grammatical gender in native Spanish speakers. Psychophysiology, E13737.Google Scholar
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Dussias, P. (2019). Revisiting masculine and feminine grammatical gender in Spanish: Linguistic, psycholinguistic, and neurolinguistic evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 751. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bellamy, K., Parafita Couto, M. C., & Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H. (2018). Investigating gender assignment strategies in mixed Purepecha – Spanish nominal constructions. Languages, 3, 28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Eberhard, K. (1993). Meaning, sound, and syntax in English number agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 57–99. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 45–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Boers, I., Sterken, B., van Osch, B., Parafita Couto, M. C., Grijzenhout, J., & Tat, D. (2020). Gender in unilingual and mixed speech of Spanish heritage speakers in the Netherlands. Languages, 5(4), 68. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohnacker, U. (2004). Nominal phrases. In G. Josefsson, C. Platzack, & G. Håkanson (Eds.), The acquisition of Swedish grammar (pp. 195–260). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., & Barber, H. A. (2015). Does the ending matter? The role of gender-to-ending consistency in sentence reading. Brain Research, 1605, 83–92. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caffarra, S., Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A. (2014). Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia, 63(oct), 124–134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carminati, M. N. (2005). Processing reflexes of the feature hierarchy (person > number > gender) and implications for linguistic theory. Lingua, 115, 259–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1939). Determining and numerating adjectives in children’s speech. Child Development, 10, 215–229. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carstens, V. (2000). Concord in minimalist theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 319–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. (1986). Acquisition of Romance, with special reference to French. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: The data (Vol. 1, pp. 687–782). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clegg, J. H. (2011). A frequency-based analysis of the norms for Spanish noun gender. Hispania, 94(2), 303–319. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colantoni, L., Martínez, R., Mazzaro, N., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Rinaldi, N. (2020). A phonetic account of Spanish – English bilinguals’ divergence with agreement. Languages, 5(4), 58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Corbett, G. (1991). Gender. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Pérez-Tattam, R. (2015). Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature re-assembly approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(1), 1–19.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. (2000). Bantu noun class systems: Loan word and acquisition evidence of semantic productivity. In G. Senft (Ed.), Classification systems (pp. 270–292). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Domínguez, A., Cuetos, F., & Segui, J. (1999). The processing of grammatical gender and number in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistics, 28, 485–498. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dorian, N. (1981). Language death: The life cycle of a Scottish Gaelic dialect. University of Pennsylvania Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eddington, D. (2002). Spanish gender assignment in an analogical framework. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 9, 49–75. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eddington, D., & Hualde, J. I. (2008). El abudante agua: Hermaphroditic Spanish nouns. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 1, 3–31. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Embick, D., & Noyer, R. (2007). Distributed Morphology and the syntax/morphology interface. In G. Ramchand & C. Reiss (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces (pp. 289–324). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, Z. (2022). Facilitative use of grammatical gender in Heritage Spanish. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12(6), 845–871. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, Z., Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2015). The differential representation of number and gender in Spanish. The Linguistic Review, 32, 703–737. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goebel-Mahrle, T., & Shin, N. L. (2020). A corpus study of child heritage speakers’ Spanish gender agreement. The International Journal of Bilingualism: Cross-disciplinary, Cross-linguistic Studies of Language Behavior, 24(5–6), 1088–1104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gómez Torrego, L. (1991). Manual de español correcto. Arco Libros.Google Scholar
Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or a real-time processing problem? Second Language Research, 28(2), 191–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harley, H., & Ritter, E. (2002). Person and number in pronouns: A feature-geometric analysis. Language, 78, 482–526. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, J. (1991). The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22(1), 27–62.Google Scholar
Hernandez Pina, F. (1984). Teoría psicosociolingüística y su aplicación a la adquisición del español como lengua materna. Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
Hur, E., Lopez Otero, J. C., & Sanchez, L. (2020). Gender agreement and assignment in Spanish heritage speakers: Does frequency matter? Languages, 5(4), 48. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson Fowler, C. (2017). Cross-language interaction during language production in L1 German – L2 Italian bilinguals (Unpublished PhD dissertation). The Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Kempe, V., & Brooks, P. J. (2001). The role of diminutives in Russian gender learning: Can child-directed speech facilitate the acquisition of inflectional morphology? Language Learning, 51(2), 221–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kozminska, K. (2015). Language contact in the Polish-American community in Chicago. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(3), 239–258. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramer, R. (2015). The morphosyntax of gender. Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T. (2021). Heritage languages in Europe. In S. Montrul & M. Polinsky (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of heritage languages and linguistics (pp. 45–68). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., & Rothman, J. (2018). Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22, 564–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lardiere, D. (2009). Some thoughts on a contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 25, 173–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lázaro Carreter, F. (1997). El dardo en la palabra. Galaxia Gutenburg/Círculo de Lectores.Google Scholar
Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Sciences, 18, 193–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Liceras, J., Fernandez Fuertes, R., Perales, S., Pérez-Tattam, R., & Spradlin K. T. (2008). Gender and gender agreement in bilingual native and non-native grammars: Child and adult functional-lexical mixings. Lingua, 118, 827–851. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, D. (2020). Born to parse: How children select their languages. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lipski, J. (1994). Latin American Spanish. Longman.Google Scholar
Lleó, C. (1998). Proto-articles in the acquisition of Spanish: Interface between phonology and morphology. In R. Fabri, A. Ortmann, & T. Parodi (Eds.), Modelle der Flexion: 18. Jahrestagung der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft (pp. 175–195). Niemeyer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lohndal, T., & Putnam, M. (2021). The tale of two lexicons: Decomposing complexity across a distributed lexicon. Heritage Language Journal, 18, 1–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
López-Ornat, S. (1997). What lies in between a pre-grammatical and a grammatical representation? Evidence on nominal and verbal form-function mappings in Spanish from 1;7 to 2;1. In A. T. Perez-Leroux & W. R. Glass (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on the acquisition of Spanish, Vol 1: Developing grammars (pp. 3–20). Cascadilla.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
(2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mariscal, S. (1996). Adquisiciones morfosintácticas en torno al sintagma nominal: El genero gramatical en Español. In M. Pérez-Pereira (Ed.), Estudios sobre la adquisicion del Castellano, Catalan, Euskera y Gallego (pp. 263–272). Universidade de Santiago de Compostela.Google Scholar
(2001). Es “a pie” equivalente a DET + N? Sobre el conocimiento temprano de las categorias gramaticales. Cognitiva, 13(1), 35–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2009). Early acquisition of gender agreement in the Spanish noun phrase: Starting small. Journal of Child Language, 36, 143–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marrero, V., Aguirre, C., & Albala, M. (2007). The acquisition of diminutives in Spanish. In I. Savickienė & W. Dressler (Eds.), The acquisition of diminutives: A crosslinguistic perspective (pp. 155–184). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mitrofanova, N., Rodina, Y., Urek, O., & Westergaard, M. (2018). Bilinguals’ sensitivity to grammatical gender cues in Russian: The role of cumulative input, proficiency, and dominance. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montes-Alcalá, C., & Shin, N. L. (2011). Las keys versus el key: Feminine gender assignment in mixed-language texts. Spanish in Context, 8(1), 119–143. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2020). Lexical and structural activation in heritage language grammars. Lecture and commentary at the Twelfth Heritage Language Research Institute. Pennsylvania State University, June 11, 2020.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Davidson, J., de la Fuente, I., & Foote, R. (2014). Early language experience facilitates the processing of gender agreement in Spanish heritage speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(1), 118–138. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., de la Fuente, I., Davidson, J., & Foote, R. (2013). The role of experience in the acquisition and production of diminutives and gender in Spanish: Evidence from L2 learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research, 29(1), 87–118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S. (2008). Gender agreement in adult second language learners and Spanish heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58(3), 503–553. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K. (2007). Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish – English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11(3), 301–328. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (2002). Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. In R. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 207–219). Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. (1994). Gender and number agreement within DP. In J. M. Meisel (Ed.), Bilingual first language acquisition: French and German grammatical development (pp. 53–88). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Norris, M. (2017). Description and analyses of nominal concord (Pt I-II). Language and Linguistics Compass, 11(11). (pt I); 10.1111/lnc3.12267 (pt II). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oh, J., Au T. K.-F., Jun, S.-A., & Lee, R. M. (2019). Childhood language memory in adult heritage language (re)learners. In M. S. Schmid & B. Köpke (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language attrition (pp. 481–492). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1989). The acquisition of morphemes: Some evidence from Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(3), 289–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1991). The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18(3), 571–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Picallo, S. (1991). Nominals and nominalization in Catalan. Probus, 3, 279–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (1997). Slot and frame patterns and the development of the determiner category. Technical report.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 456–463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14, 191–262.Google Scholar
(2018). Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(in press). Syntactic properties of heritage grammars. In E. Leivada & K. K. Grohmann (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of minimalism and its applications. Cambridge University Press.
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2020). Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 3–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & White, L. (2000). Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Second Language Research, 16(1), 103–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M. T. (2020). Lexicalizing exponents in multilingual grammars. Second Language Research, 36(2), 187–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M. T., & Sánchez, L. (2013). What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition? A prolegomenon to modeling heritage language grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(4), 478–508. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Putnam, M. T., Schwarz, L. S., & Homan, A. D. (2021). Morphology. In S. Montrul & M. Polinsky (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of heritage languages and linguistics (pp. 613–643). Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Radford, A., Kupisch, T., Koppe, R., & Gabriele Azzarro, G. (2007). Concord, convergence and accommodation in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(3), 239–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Riksem, B. R., Grimstad, M., Lohndal, T., & Åfarli, T. A. (2019). Language mixing within verbs and nouns in American Norwegian. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 22(2), 189–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ritter, E. (1991). Two functional categories in noun phrases: Evidence from Modern Hebrew. In S. Rothstein (Ed.), Perspectives on phrase structure (pp. 37–62). Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1993). Where’s gender? Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 795–803.Google Scholar
Rodina, Y., Kupisch, T., Meir, N., Mitrofanova, N., Urek, O., & Westergaard, M. (2020). Internal and external factors in heritage language acquisition: Evidence from heritage Russian in Israel, Germany, Norway, Latvia and the United Kingdom. Frontiers in Education, 11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rothman, J. (2009). Understanding the nature and outcomes of early bilingualism: Romance languages as heritage languages. International Journal of Bilingualism, 13, 155–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Sadek, C., Kiraithe, J., & Villarreal, H. (1975). The acquisition of the concept of grammatical gender in monolingual and bilingual speakers of Spanish. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Washington, D.C., April 1, 1975.Google Scholar
Sankoff, G. (2001). Linguistic outcomes of language contact. In P. Trudgill, J. Chambers, & N. Schilling-Estes (Eds.), Handbook of sociolinguistics (pp. 638–668). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Savickienė, I., & Dressler, W. (Eds.) (2007). The acquisition of diminutives: A crosslinguistic perspective. John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., & Fuchs, Z. (2018). In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa, 3(1), 1. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I. (2012). Real-time processing of gender and number agreement by native and heritage Russian speakers. Paper presented at the Workshop on Formal Approaches to Heritage Languages, UMass Amherst, 21 April 2012. Retrieved on 10 November 2023 from [URL]Google Scholar
Seva, N., Kempe, V., Brooks, P. J., Mironova, N., & Pershukova, A. (2007). Crosslinguistic evidence for the diminutive advantage: Gender agreement in Russian and Serbian children. Journal of Child Language, 34, 111–131. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shin, N. L., Rodriguez, B. L., Armijo, A., & Perara-Lunde, M. (2019). Child heritage speakers’ production and comprehension of direct object clitic gender in Spanish. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9(4/5), 659–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Silva-Corvalán, C. (1989). CHILDES Catalan – Spanish Silva-Corvalán corpus. Retrieved on 10 November 2023 from [URL]
Smith, P., Nix, A., Davey, N., López Ornat, S., & Messer, D. (2003). A connectionist account of Spanish determiner production. Journal of Child Language, 30, 305–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snyder, W., Senghas, A., & Inman, K. (2001). Agreement morphology and the acquisition of noun-drop in Spanish. Language Acquisition, 9(2), 157–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Soler, R. (1984). Acquisicion y utilizacion del articulo. In M. Siguan (Ed.), Estudios sobre psicologia del lenguaje infantil (pp. 139–166). Piramide.Google Scholar
Svenonius, P. (2016). Spans and words. In H. Harley & D. Siddiqi (Eds.), Morphological metatheory (pp. 199–220). John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Declension class and the Norwegian definite suffix. In V. Gribanova & S. Shih (Eds.), The morphosyntax-phonology connection: Locality and directionality at the interface. Oxford Scholarship Online. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Teschner, R. V., & Russell, W. M. (1984). The gender patterns of Spanish nouns: An inverse dictionary-based analysis. Hispanic Linguistics, 1, 115-132.Google Scholar
Thomason, S. G., & Kaufman, T. (1988). Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. University of California Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 156–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., Sorace, A., Heycock, C., & Filiaci, F. (2004). First language attrition and syntactic subjects: A study of Greek and Italian near-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 8, 257–277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valdés Kroff, J. R., Dussias, P. E., Gerfen, C., Perrotti, L., & Bajo, M. T. (2017). Experience with code-switching modulates the use of grammatical gender during sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(2), 163–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, E., Faure, A., Ramírez-Trujillo, A. P., Barski, E., Pangtay, Y., & Diez, A. (2012). Gender and heritage Spanish bilingual grammars: A study of code-mixed Determiner Phrases and copula constructions. Hispania, 95(3), 481–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
White, L. (2011). Second language acquisition at the interfaces. Lingua, 121, 577–590. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
White, L., Valenzuela, E., Kozlowska-Macgregor, M., & Leung, Y.-K. I. (2004). Gender agreement in non-native Spanish: Evidence against failed features. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105–133. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wicha, N. Y. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2004). Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1272–1288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. (2005). On productivity. Linguistic Variation Yearbook, 5(1), 265–302. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2016). The price of linguistic productivity: How children learn to break rules of language. The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar