References (110)
References
Abdul Aziz, S., Fletcher, J., & Bayliss, D. M. (2016). The effectiveness of self-regulatory speech training for planning and problem solving in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(6), 1045–1059. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Self-regulatory speech during planning and problem-solving in children with SLI and their typically developing peers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 52(3), 311–322. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Acar, S., & Runco, M. (2019). Divergent thinking through cognitive hyperspace for creative ideas. In L. Dorfman, P. Machotka, & V. Petrov (Eds.), Integrative explorations of the creative mind (pp. 40–62). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M., Hadley, C. M., & Kramer, S. J. (2002). Creativity under the gun. Harvard Business Review, 52–61.Google Scholar
American Councils Research Center (2021). 2021 Canvass of dual language and immersion (DLI) programs in US Public Schools. Retrieved on 13 December 2023 from [URL]
Amit, E., & Greene, J. D. (2012). You see, the ends don’t justify the means: Visual imagery and moral judgment. Psychological Science, 23(8), 861–868. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. A. E., Chung-Fat-Yim, A., Bellana, B., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2018). Language and cognitive control networks in bilinguals and monolinguals. Neuropsychologia, 117, 352–363. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Androutsopoulos, J. (2020). Trans-scripting as a multilingual practice: The case of Hellenised English. International Journal of Multilingualism, 17(3), 286–308. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barke, E. M., & Williams, D. P. (1938). A further study of the comparative intelligence of children in certain bilingual and monoglot schools in South Wales. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 8(1), 63–77. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Hawrylewicz, K., Grundy, J. G., & Chung-Fat-Yim, A. (2022). The swerve: How childhood bilingualism changed from liability to benefit. Developmental Psychology, 58(8), 1429–1440. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok E., & Shapero D. (2005). Ambiguous benefits: The effect of bilingualism on reversing ambiguous figures. Developmental Science, 8, 595–604. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Booton, S., Hoicka, E., O’Grady, A., Chan, H. Y. N., & Murphy, V. (2021). Children’s divergent thinking and bilingualism. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 41, 100918. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bull, R., & Lee, K. (2014). Executive functioning and mathematics achievement. Child Development Perspectives, 8(1), 36–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Collins, M. A., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Motivation and creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 297–312). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18(3), 391–404. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cushen, P. J., & Wiley, J. (2011). Aha! Voila! Eureka! Bilingualism and insightful problem solving. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 458–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
de Bono, E. (1967). New think: The use of lateral thinking in the generation of new ideas. Basic Books.Google Scholar
DiNapoli, J., & Hector Morales, J. (2021). Translanguaging to persevere is key for latinx bilinguals’ mathematical success. Journal of Urban Mathematics Education, 14(2), 71–104. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Duchêne, A., & Heller, M. (2012). Multilingualism and the new economy. In M. Martin-Hones, A. Blackledge, & A. Creese (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of multilingualism (1st ed., pp. 369–383). Routledge.Google Scholar
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), i–113. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edl, S., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). Creativity and the Stroop interference effect. Personality and individual Differences, 69, 38–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2020). Problem solving and expertise. In M. W. Eysenck & M. T. Keane (Eds.), Cognitive psychology (8th ed., pp. 573–621). Psychology Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Federal Statistical Office (2021). Switzerland’s population in 2021. Retrieved on 13 December 2023 from [URL]
Fernandez-Duque, M., Hayakawa, S., & Marian, V. (2023). Speakers of different languages remember visual scenes differently. Science Advances, 9(33), eadh0064. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Foreman-Peck, J., & Wang, Y. (2014). ‘The costs to the UK of language deficiencies as a barrier to UK engagement in exporting: A report to UK Trade & Investment.’ UK Trade and Investment (UKTI). Retrieved on 13 December 2023 from [URL]
Gangopadhyay, I., McDonald, M., Ellis Weismer, S., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2018). Planning abilities in bilingual and monolingual children: Role of verbal mediation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 323. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
García, O. (2018). Translanguaging, pedagogy and creativity. In J. Erfurt, E. Carporal, & A. Weirich (Eds.), Éducation plurilingue et pratiques langagières: Hommage à Christine Hélot (pp. 39–56). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2015). The foreign language effect on moral judgment: The role of emotions and norms. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0131529. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Weisberg, R. W. (1966). Verbal behavior and problem solving: Some effects of labeling in a functional fixedness problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(5), 659–664. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goel, V. (2010). Neural basis of thinking: Laboratory problems versus real-world problems. WIREs Cognitive Science, 1(4), 613–621. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goff, K., & Torrance, E. P. (2002). Abbreviated Torrance test for adults. Scholastic Testing Service.Google Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Fennema-Notestine, C., & Morris, S. K. (2005). Bilingualism affects picture naming but not picture classification. Memory and Cognition, 33(7), 1220–1234. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive Development, 28(1), 41–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grin, F., Sfreddo, C., & Vaillancourt, F. (2010). The contribution of multilingualism to value creation. In F. Grin, C. Sfreddo, & F. Vaillancourt (Eds.), The economics of the multilingual workplace (1st ed., pp. 105–122). Routledge.Google Scholar
Guilford J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hartanto, A., Yang, H., & Yang, S. (2018). Bilingualism positively predicts mathematical competence: Evidence from two large-scale studies. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 216–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Costa, A., Foucart, A., & Keysar, B. (2016). Using a foreign language changes our choices. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(11), 791–793. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., & Keysar, B. (2018). Using a foreign language reduces mental imagery. Cognition, 173, 8–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayakawa, S., Lau, B. K. Y., Holtzmann, S., Costa, A., & Keysar, B. (2017). On the reliability of the foreign language effect on risk-taking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(1), 29–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T., & Chaires, W. M. (1980). Accessibility of interrelational constructs: Implications for stimulus encoding and creativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16(4), 348–361. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, I., & Costa, A. (2008). Does bilingualism hamper lexical access in speech production? Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 277–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jasper, J. (2018). The transformative limits of translanguaging. Language and Communication, 58, 1–10. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jiang, W. (2000). The relationship between culture and language. ELT journal, 54(4), 328–334. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jonassen, D. H., & Hung, W. (2012). Problem solving. In N. M. Seel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the sciences of learning (pp. 2680–2683). Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kempert, S., Saalbach, H., & Hardy, I. (2011). Cognitive benefits and costs of bilingualism in elementary school students: The case of mathematical word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 547–561. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kharkhurin, A. V. (2008). The effects of linguistic proficiency, age of second language acquisition, and length of exposure to a new cultural environment on bilinguals’ divergent thinking. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(2), 225–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010a). Sociocultural differences in the relationship between bilingualism and creative potential. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(5–6), 776–783. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2010b). Bilingual verbal and nonverbal creative behavior. International Journal of Bilingualism, 14, 211–226. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2012). Multilingualism and creativity. Multilingual Matters. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2011). The role of selective attention in bilingual creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23(3), 239–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017). Language mediated concept activation in bilingual memory facilitates cognitive flexibility. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kharkhurin, A. V., & Wei, L. (2015). The role of code-switching in bilingual creativity. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18(2), 153–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kramsch, C. (2014). Language and culture. AILA Review, 27(1), 30–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., Valdes-Kroff, J. (2012). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 229–262). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lange, K. V., Hopman, E. W. M., Zemla, J. C., & Austerweil, J. L. (2020). Evidence against a relation between bilingualism and creativity. PLoS ONE, 15(6), e0234928. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lee, H., & Kim, K. H. (2011). Can speaking more languages enhance your creativity? Relationship between bilingualism and creative potential among Korean American students with multicultural link. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(8), 1186–1190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leikin, M. (2012). The effect of bilingualism on creativity: Developmental and educational perspectives. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 431–447. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leikin, M., & Tovli, E. (2014). Bilingualism and creativity in early childhood. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 411–417. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. -Y (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63(3), 169–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Li, W. (2013). Codeswitching. In R. Bayley, R. Cameron, & C. Lucas (Eds.), Oxford handbook of sociolinguistics (pp. 360–378). Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lin, A. (2013). Classroom code-switching: Three decades of research. Applied Linguistics Review, 4, 195–218. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lu, J. G., Hafenbrack, A. C., Eastwick, P. W., Wang, D. J., Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2017). “Going out” of the box: Close intercultural friendships and romantic relationships spark creativity, workplace innovation, and entrepreneurship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(7), 1091–1108. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. (1999). Creativity across cultures. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 339–350). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J., & Grady, C. (2011). Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(10), 1479–1488. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28(1), 28–34. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Maddux, W. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Cultural borders and mental barriers: The relationship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1047–1061. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V. (2023). The power of language: How the codes we use to think, speak, and live transform our minds. Dutton. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Shook, A., & Schroeder, S. R. (2013). Bilingual two-way immersion programs benefit academic achievement. Bilingual Research Journal, 36(2), 167–186. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97–115. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Marsh, L. G., & Maki, R. H. (1976). Efficiency of arithmetic operations in bilinguals as a function of language. Memory and Cognition, 4(4), 459–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McClain, L., & Huang, J. Y. (1982). Speed of simple arithmetic in bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 10(6), 591–596. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKinlay, A. (2011). Tower tasks. In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.) Encyclopedia of child behavior and development. Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220–232. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1968). Remote Associates Test. Journal of Creative Behavior, 2, 213–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ning, S., Hayakawa, S., Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V. (2020). On language and thought: Bilingual experience influences semantic associations. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, 100932. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The philosophical roots of Western and Eastern conceptions of creativity. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 26(1–2), 18–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Olson, J. A., Nahas, J., Chmoulevitch, D., Cropper, S. J., & Webb, M. E. (2021). Naming unrelated words predicts creativity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(25), e2022340118. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Omidire, M. F., & Ayob, S. (2022). The utilisation of translanguaging for learning and teaching in multilingual primary classrooms. Multilingua, 41(1), 105–129. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Otheguy, R., García, O., & Reid, W. (2015). Clarifying translanguaging and deconstructing named languages: A perspective from linguistics. Applied Linguistics Review, 6(3), 281–307. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panović, I. (2018). ‘You don’t have enough letters to make this noise’: Arabic speakers’ creative engagements with the Roman script. Language Sciences, 65, 70–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradowski, M. B. (2021). Transitions, translanguaging, trans-semiotising in heteroglossic school environments: Lessons from (not only) South African classrooms. In C. van der Walt & V. Pfeiffer (Eds.), Multilingual classroom contexts: Transitions and transactions (pp. 213–284). SUN Press.Google Scholar
Peal, E., & Lambert, W. E. (1962). The relation of bilingualism to intelligence. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 76(27), 1–23. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Puryear, J. S., & Lamb, K. N. (2020) Defining creativity: How far have we come since Plucker, Beghetto, and Dow? Creativity Research Journal, 32(3), 206–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ricciardelli, L. A. (1992a). Creativity and bilingualism. Journal of Creative Behavior, 26(4), 242–254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1992b). Bilingualism and cognitive development in relation to threshold theory. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 21(4), 301–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and culture: A two way interaction. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47(3), 273–290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rudowicz, E., & Yue, X. D. (2000). Concepts of creativity: Similarities and differences among mainland, Hong Kong and Taiwanese Chinese. Journal of Creative Behavior, 34(3), 175–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saer, D. J. (1923). The effects of bilingualism on intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 14, 25–38.Google Scholar
Sampedro, A., & Peña, J. (2019). Executive functions mediate the association between bilingualism and creativity in preadolescents. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 34, 100605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shao, Y., Zhang, C., Zhou, J., Gu, T., & Yuan, Y. (2019) How does culture shape creativity? A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1219. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shook, A., & Marian, V. (2013). The bilingual language interaction network for comprehension of speech. Bilingualism, 16(2), 304–324. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019). Covert co-activation of bilinguals’ non-target language. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 9(2), 228–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K., & Damian, R. I. (2012). Creativity. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. (2003). What makes us smart? Core knowledge and natural language. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 277–311). The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Storme, M., Çelik, P., Camargo, A., Forthmann, B., Holling, H., & Lubart, T. (2017). The effect of forced language switching during divergent thinking: A study on bilinguals’ originality of ideas. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2086. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1966). Torrance test of creative thinking. Personnel Press.Google Scholar
van Dijk, M., Kroesbergen, E. H., Blom, E., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2018). Bilingualism and creativity: Towards a situated cognition approach. Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(2), 178–188. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Rinsveld, A., Dricot, L., Guillaume, M., Rossion, B., & Schiltz, C. (2017). Mental arithmetic in the bilingual brain: Language matters. Neuropsychologia, 101, 17–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wallace, G. L., Peng, C. S., & Williams, D. (2017). Interfering with inner speech selectively disrupts problem solving and is linked with real-world executive functioning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(12), 3456–3460. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wei, L., & Hua, Z. (2019). Tranßcripting: Playful subversion with Chinese characters. International Journal of Multilingualism, 16(2), 145–161. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wei, L., Tsang, A., Wong, N., & Lok, P. (2020). Kongish Daily: Researching translanguaging creativity and subversiveness. International Journal of Multilingualism, 17(3), 309–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, R. W., & Suls, J. M. (1973). An information-processing model of Duncker’s candle problem. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 255–276. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wimmer M. C., & Marx C. (2014). Inhibitory processes in visual perception: A bilingual advantage. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 412–419. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
WIPO (2021). Global Innovation Index 2021: Tracking Innovation through the COVID-19 Crisis. World Intellectual Property Organization.Google Scholar
Xia, T., An, Y., & Guo, J. (2022). Bilingualism and creativity: Benefits from cognitive inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1016777. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Lim, T. Z. M. (2019). Sharing the same languages helps us work better together. Palgrave Communications, 5, 154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zabelina, D. L., Friedman, N. P., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2019). Unity and diversity of executive functions in creativity. Consciousness and Cognition, 68, 47–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar