References (47)
References
Barac, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development, 83(2), 413–422. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word, 14, 150–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 3–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 67–73. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522–538. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Sujin, Y. A. N. G. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–531. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Peets, K. F., & Moreno, S. (2014). Producing bilinguals through immersion education: Development of metalinguistic awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(1), 177–191. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Jessen, A. (2019). Do bilingual children lag behind? A study of morphological encoding using ERPs. Journal of Child Language, 46(5), 955–979. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cojocaru, D. (2003). Romanian grammar. Slavic and East European Language Research Center (SEELRC), Duke University.Google Scholar
Cummins, J. (1978). Bilingualism and the development of metalinguistic awareness. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 9(2), 131–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Kieffer, M. J., & Laroche, A. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and reading comprehension: Evidence from mediation and longitudinal models. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(6), 432–451. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(2), 223–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., & Thornton, A. M. (1996). Italian nominal inflection. Wiener Linguistische Gazette, 57–59, 1–26.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2000). Peabody – test di vocabolario recettivo – P.P.V.T.-R. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised. Omega.Google Scholar
Fejzo, A. (2021). The contribution of morphological awareness to vocabulary among L1 and L2 French-speaking 4th-graders. Reading and Writing, 34, 659–679. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fowler, A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. In L. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 157–188). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kieffer, M. J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2012). Development of morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in Spanish-speaking language minority learners: A parallel process latent growth curve model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33(1), 23–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-language perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41(3), 161–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Leivada, E., Westergaard, M., Duñabeitia, J., & Rothman, J. (2021). On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(1), 197–210. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lenth R (2023). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (R package version 1.8.4-1. Retrieved on 18 December 2023 from [URL]Google Scholar
Melloni, C., & Vender, M. (2022). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia: Playing with nonwords in a morphologically rich language. PLOS ONE, 17(11), e0276643. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Melloni, C., Vender, M., & Delfitto, D. (2019). Inflectional morphology: Evidence for an advantage of bilingualism in Albanian-Italian and Romanian-Italian bilingual children. In R. Slabakova, J. Corbet, L. Dominguez, A. Dudley, & A. Wallington (Eds.), Explorations in second language acquisition and processing (pp. 238–250). Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 134. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newmark, L., Hubbard, P., & Prifti, P. R. (1982). Standard Albanian: A reference grammar for students. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., & Bowles, H. (1998). Acquiring regular and irregular inflection in a language with verb classes. Language & Cognitive Processes, 13, 425–464. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2011). Bilingual children’s acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 38(3), 554–578. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Park, J., Wiseheart, R., & Ritter, M. (2014). The roles of the speed and accuracy of morphological processing in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking language minority learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature, 3(1), 12–27. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Piccinin, S., & Dal Maso, S. (2022). Sviluppare la consapevolezza morfologica: Linee guida per un intervento didattico. Studi di Glottodidattica, 3, 85–94.Google Scholar
(2023), Investigating factors affecting reading comprehension in second-generation pupils: The role of morphological skills. Lingue e Linguaggio, XXII(1), 117–142.Google Scholar
Pirrelli, V., & Battista, M. (2000). On the interaction of paradigmatic and syntagmatic stem alternation in Italian conjugation. Acta Linguistica Hungarica, 47(1–4), 289–314. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Luo, Y. (2011). Morphological awareness and word reading in English language learners: Evidence from Spanish- and Chinese-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(3), 601–618. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raven, J., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. C. 1998. Raven manual, Section 1 (General overview) and Section 2 (Coloured progressive matrices). Oxford Psychologist Press.Google Scholar
Rispens, J. & De Bree, E. (2015). Bilingual children’s production of regular and irregular past tense morphology. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(2), 290–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shahar-Yames, D., Eviatar, Z., & Prior, A. (2018). Separability of lexical and morphological knowledge: Evidence from language minority children. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–15. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singson, M., Mahoney, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 219–252. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sparks, E., & Deacon, H. S. (2015). Morphological awareness and vocabulary acquisition: A longitudinal examination of their relationship in English-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 299–321. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stella, G., Pizzioli, C., & Tressoldi, P. (2000). PEABODY – Test di vocabolario recettivo – P.P.V.T. – R. Peabody picture vocabulary test – revised. Omega Edizioni.Google Scholar
Stella, V., & Job, R. (2001). Le sillabe PD/DPSS. Una base di dati sulla frequenza dell’italiano scritto. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 3, 633–639.Google Scholar
Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(3), 523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vender, M., Garraffa, M., Sorace, A., & Guasti, M. T. (2016). How early L2 children perform on Italian clinical markers of SLI: A study of clitic production and nonword repetition. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 30(2), 150–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vender, M., Hu, S., Mantione, F., Savazzi, S., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2021). Inflectional morphology: Evidence for an advantage of bilingualism in dyslexia. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 24(2), 155–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vender, M., Mantione, F., Savazzi, S., Delfitto, D., & Melloni, C. (2017). Inflectional morphology and dyslexia: Italian children’s performance in a nonword pluralization task. Annals of Dyslexia, 67, 401–426. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., Voeten, M., & Vermeer, A. (2019). Beyond the simple view of early second language reading: The impact of lexical quality. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 28–36. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wu, Z., & Juffs, A. (2022). Effects of L1 morphological type on L2 morphological awareness. Second Language Research, 38(4), 787–812. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yan, S., & Nicoladis, E. (2009). Finding le mot juste: Differences between bilingual and monolingual children’s lexical access in comprehension and production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(3), 323–335. DOI logoGoogle Scholar