Chapter published in:
Narrative, Literacy and Other Skills: Studies in intervention
Edited by Edy Veneziano and Ageliki Nicolopoulou
[Studies in Narrative 25] 2019
► pp. 129149
References
Adrían, J. E., Clemente, R. A., & Villanueva, L.
(2007) Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78(4), 1052–1067. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Adrían, J. E., Clementeß, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C.
(2005) Parent–child picture-book reading, mothers’ mental state language and children’s theory of mind. Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 673–686. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Aram, D., Fine, Y., & Ziv, M.
(2013) Enhancing parent–child shared book reading interactions: Promoting references to the book’s plot and socio-cognitive themes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 111–122. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brockmeyer, C. Nicolopoulou, A., de Sá, A., Ilgaz, H., & Cortina, K. S.
(2008) Multitasking: The best way to assess preschoolers’ narrative competence. Paper presented at the XI International Association for the Study of Child Language. Edinburgh, UK.Google Scholar
Bruner, J.
(1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chandler, M., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S.
(1989) Small-scale deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-year-olds’ theories of mind. Child Development, 60(6), 1263–1277. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier, C.
(1987) Spence is small. Part Ridge, IL: Albert Whitman & Co.Google Scholar
Cole, K., & Mitchell, P.
(2001) Family background in relation to deceptive ability and understanding of the mind. Social Development, 7(2), 181–197. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Curenton, S. M.
(2003) Low-income preschoolers’ false-belief performance. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(4), 411–424. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dyer, J. R., Shatz, M., & Wellman, H. M.
(2000) Young children’s storybooks as a source of mental state information. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 17–37. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ehrlich, H. M.
(2000) Louie’s goose. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L.
(1983) Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 95–120. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, K. L., & Reese, E.
(2005) Picture book reading with young children: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 25(1), 64–103. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Galdone, P.
(1985) The little red hen. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A.
(2004) Test of narrative language: Examiner’s manual. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Goodsitt, J., Raitan, J. G., & Perlmutter, M.
(1988) Interaction between mothers and preschool children when reading a novel and familiar book. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(4), 489–505. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guajardo, N. R., & Watson, A. C.
(2002) Narrative discourse and theory of mind development. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 305–325. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L.
(2006) Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind. In J. W. Astington, & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp.70–83). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, H., Black, C., & Miller, S.
(1996) A cross-task comparison of false belief understanding in a Head Start population. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(2), 263–285. Retrieved from http://​www​.ncbi​.nlm​.nih​.gov​/pubmed​/8954606 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D.
(2008) Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kasza, K.
(2005) My lucky day. New York, NY: Puffin Books.Google Scholar
Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J.
(1995) False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2–3), 167–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K.
(2008) The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(3), 173–192. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.
(2007) Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peskin, J., & Astington, J. W.
(2004) The effects of adding metacognitive language to story texts. Cognitive Development, 19(2), 253–273. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, G., & McNaughton, S.
(1990) The practice of storybook reading in mainstream New Zealand Families. Reading Research Quarterly, 25(3), 196–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Racine, T. P., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Turnbull, W.
(2007) Parent–child talk and children’s understanding of beliefs and emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 21(3), 480–494. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Rall, J., & Harris, P. L.
(2000) In Cinderella’s slippers? Story comprehension from the protagonist’s point of view. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 202–208. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ratner, N. K., & Olver, R. R.
(1998) Reading a tale of deception, learning a theory of mind? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 219–239. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, D., & Saunders, J.
(1990) Dibble and Dabble. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster Children’s Publishing.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Diesendruck, G., Martinez-Beck, I., & Akar, D.
(2003) The influence of language and socioeconomic status on children’s understanding of false belief. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 717–729. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Society for Personality and Social Psychology
(2014, August 11) Can fiction stories make us more empathetic? Retrieved from www​.sciencedaily​.com​/releases​/2014​/08​/140811151632​.htm.
Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E.
(2005) Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81–102. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D.
(2004) Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. M.
(2004) CELF Preschool 2: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool. Pearson/PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Bergman Deitcher, Deborah, Dorit Aram, Mabsam Khalaily-Shahadi & Mona Dwairy
2021. Promoting Preschoolers’ Mental-Emotional Conceptualization and Social Understanding: A Shared Book-Reading Study. Early Education and Development 32:4  pp. 501 ff. Crossref logo
Timperley, Sarah, Elizabeth Schaughency, Ruby-Rose McDonald & Elaine Reese
2020. Rhymes and Relatability: How Storybook Style and Content Relate to Home-Based Educators’ Extra-Textual Talk. Early Education and Development  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 08 may 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.