Part of
Narrative, Literacy and Other Skills: Studies in intervention
Edited by Edy Veneziano and Ageliki Nicolopoulou
[Studies in Narrative 25] 2019
► pp. 129149
References
Adrían, J. E., Clemente, R. A., & Villanueva, L.
(2007) Mothers’ use of cognitive state verbs in picture-book reading and the development of children’s understanding of mind: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 78(4), 1052–1067. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adrían, J. E., Clementeß, R. A., Villanueva, L., & Rieffe, C.
(2005) Parent–child picture-book reading, mothers’ mental state language and children’s theory of mind. Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 673–686. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aram, D., Fine, Y., & Ziv, M.
(2013) Enhancing parent–child shared book reading interactions: Promoting references to the book’s plot and socio-cognitive themes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(1), 111–122. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brockmeyer, C. Nicolopoulou, A., de Sá, A., Ilgaz, H., & Cortina, K. S.
(2008) Multitasking: The best way to assess preschoolers’ narrative competence. Paper presented at the XI International Association for the Study of Child Language. Edinburgh, UK.Google Scholar
Bruner, J.
(1986) Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chandler, M., Fritz, A. S., & Hala, S.
(1989) Small-scale deceit: Deception as a marker of two-, three-, and four-year-olds’ theories of mind. Child Development, 60(6), 1263–1277. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chevalier, C.
(1987) Spence is small. Part Ridge, IL: Albert Whitman & Co.Google Scholar
Cole, K., & Mitchell, P.
(2001) Family background in relation to deceptive ability and understanding of the mind. Social Development, 7(2), 181–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curenton, S. M.
(2003) Low-income preschoolers’ false-belief performance. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164(4), 411–424. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dyer, J. R., Shatz, M., & Wellman, H. M.
(2000) Young children’s storybooks as a source of mental state information. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 17–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ehrlich, H. M.
(2000) Louie’s goose. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L.
(1983) Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 95–120. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, K. L., & Reese, E.
(2005) Picture book reading with young children: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 25(1), 64–103. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Galdone, P.
(1985) The little red hen. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company.Google Scholar
Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A.
(2004) Test of narrative language: Examiner’s manual. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Goodsitt, J., Raitan, J. G., & Perlmutter, M.
(1988) Interaction between mothers and preschool children when reading a novel and familiar book. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11(4), 489–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Guajardo, N. R., & Watson, A. C.
(2002) Narrative discourse and theory of mind development. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(3), 305–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L.
(2006) Conversation, pretense, and theory of mind. In J. W. Astington, & J. A. Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp.70–83). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, H., Black, C., & Miller, S.
(1996) A cross-task comparison of false belief understanding in a Head Start population. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(2), 263–285. Retrieved from [URL] DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D.
(2008) Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kasza, K.
(2005) My lucky day. New York, NY: Puffin Books.Google Scholar
Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J.
(1995) False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2–3), 167–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K.
(2008) The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science 3(3), 173–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K.
(2007) Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peskin, J., & Astington, J. W.
(2004) The effects of adding metacognitive language to story texts. Cognitive Development, 19(2), 253–273. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Phillips, G., & McNaughton, S.
(1990) The practice of storybook reading in mainstream New Zealand Families. Reading Research Quarterly, 25(3), 196–212. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Racine, T. P., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Turnbull, W.
(2007) Parent–child talk and children’s understanding of beliefs and emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 21(3), 480–494. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rall, J., & Harris, P. L.
(2000) In Cinderella’s slippers? Story comprehension from the protagonist’s point of view. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 202–208. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ratner, N. K., & Olver, R. R.
(1998) Reading a tale of deception, learning a theory of mind? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 219–239. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saunders, D., & Saunders, J.
(1990) Dibble and Dabble. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster Children’s Publishing.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Diesendruck, G., Martinez-Beck, I., & Akar, D.
(2003) The influence of language and socioeconomic status on children’s understanding of false belief. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 717–729. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Society for Personality and Social Psychology
(2014, August 11) Can fiction stories make us more empathetic? Retrieved from [URL].
Symons, D. K., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E.
(2005) Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D.
(2004) Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. M.
(2004) CELF Preschool 2: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool. Pearson/PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Bergman Deitcher, Deborah, Dorit Aram, Mabsam Khalaily-Shahadi & Mona Dwairy
2021. Promoting Preschoolers’ Mental-Emotional Conceptualization and Social Understanding: A Shared Book-Reading Study. Early Education and Development 32:4  pp. 501 ff. DOI logo
Timperley, Sarah, Elizabeth Schaughency, Ruby-Rose McDonald & Elaine Reese
2021. Rhymes and Relatability: How Storybook Style and Content Relate to Home-Based Educators’ Extra-Textual Talk. Early Education and Development 32:8  pp. 1240 ff. DOI logo
Wimmer, Lena, Gregory Currie, Stacie Friend & Heather J. Ferguson
2021. The effects of reading narrative fiction on social and moral cognition. Scientific Study of Literature 11:2  pp. 223 ff. DOI logo
Zheng, Yuanxia, Danyang Li, Zhongqi Chen & Guoxiong Liu
2022. Picture book reading on the development of preschoolers in rural areas of China: Effects on language, inhibition, and theory of mind. Frontiers in Psychology 13 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.