Part of
Narrative, Literacy and Other Skills: Studies in intervention
Edited by Edy Veneziano and Ageliki Nicolopoulou
[Studies in Narrative 25] 2019
► pp. 173199
References
Baker, N. D., & Nelson, K. E.
(1984) Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances by young children. First Language, 5, 3–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M.
(1987) The acquisition of narratives. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bamberg, M., & Damrad-Frye, R.
(1991) On the ability to provide evaluative comments: Further explorations of children’s narrative competencies. Journal of Child Language, 18, 689–710. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
(1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Berman, R.
(2004) The role of context in developing narrative abilities. In S. Strömqvist, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp.261–281). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. I.
(1994) Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowyer-Crane, C., Snowling, M. J., Duff, F. J., Fieldsend, E., Carroll, J. M., Miles, J., … Hulme, C.
(2008) Improving early language and literacy skills: Differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 422–432. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Camarata, S., & Nelson, K. E.
(2006) Conversational recast intervention with preschool and older children. In R. McCauley, & M. Fey (Ed.), Treatment of language disorders in children (pp.237–264). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Case, R., & Okamoto, Y.
(1996) The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, 1–265. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chang, C., & McCabe, A.
(2013) Evaluation in Mandarin Chinese children’s personal narratives. In A. McCAbe, & C. Chang. (Eds.), Chinese language narration: Culture, cognition, and emotion (pp.33–56). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, L., & Pan, N.
(2009) Development of English referring expressions in narratives of Chinese-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(4), 429–445. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colozzo, P., & Whitely, C.
(2014) Keeping track of characters: factors affecting referential adequacy in children’s narratives. First Language, 34(2), 155 –177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cordova, D. I., & Lepper, M. R.
(1996) Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 715–730. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L.
(2004) African American and Caucasian preschoolers’ use of decontextualized language: Use of literate language features in oral narratives. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in the Schools, 35, 240–253. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., & Platsidou, M.
(2002) The development of mental processing: efficiency, working memory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67 (1, Serial No. 268).Google Scholar
Dickinson, D., McCabe, A., Anastasopoulos, L., Peisner-Feinberg, E., & Poe, M.
(2003) The comprehensive language approach to early literacy: The interrelationships among vocabulary, phonological sensitivity, and print knowledge among preschool-aged children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 465–481. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C., & Connell, P. J.
(1996) Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(3), 611–624. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Feagans, L. V., & Farran, D. C.
(1994) The effect of daycare intervention in the preschool years on the narrative skills of poverty children in kindergarten. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17(3), 503–523. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Burack, J. A.
(1998) I. Cognitive complexity and control: Implications for theory of mind in typical and atypical development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 116–121. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
The Game of Life
(1985) The Milton Bradley Co.Google Scholar
Gillam, R., McFadden, T. U., & Kleeck, A. V.
(1995) Improving narrative abilities: Whole language and language skills approaches. In M. E. Fay, J. Windsor, & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Language intervention: preschool through the elementary years. Vol. 5 (pp.145–182). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Quinn, R.
(1993) Assessing narratives of children from diverse cultural/linguistic groups. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 24, 2–9. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S.
(1998) Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 803–864. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hayward, D. & Schneider, P.
(2000) Effectiveness of teaching story grammar knowledge to pre-school children with language impairment: An exploratory study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 16(3), 255–284. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M.
(2004) Coherence, cohesion, and context: some comparative perspectives in narrative development. In S. Strömqvist, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp.281–306). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M., Hendriks, H., Roland, F., & Liang, J.
(1996) The marking of new information in children’s narratives: a comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 23(3), 591–619. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jisa, H., Reilly, J., Verhoeven, L., Baruch, E., & Rosado, E.
(2002) Passive voice constructions in written texts: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 163–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, N. S.
(1983) What do you do if you can't tell the whole story? The development of summarization skills. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s Language, Vol. 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kail, M., & Hickmann, M.
(1992) French children’s ability to introduce referents in narratives as a function of mutual knowledge. First Language, 12, 73–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1986) Stage/structure versus phase/process in modeling linguistic and cognitive development. In I. Levin (Ed.), Stage and structure: Reopening the debate (pp.164–190). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Karmiloff K., & Karmiloff-Smith A.
(2001) Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.Google Scholar
Khan, K. S., Nelson, K. E., & Whyte, E. M.
(2014) Children choose their own stories: The impact of choice on children’s learning of new narrative skills. Journal of Child Language, 41(4), 949–962. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lai, W.-F.
(2013) Socioeconomic differences in Taiwanese children’s personal narratives: Conjunctions, internal state terms, and narrative structures. In A. McCabe, & C. Chang (Eds.), Chinese language narration: Culture, cognition, and emotion (pp.115–142). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynch, J. S., Van den Broek, P. W., Kremer, K. E., Kendeou, P., White, M. J., & Lorch, E. P.
(2008) The development of narrative comprehension and its relation to other early reading skills. Reading Psychology, 29 (4), 327–365. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, N. S.
(1977) Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9(1), 111–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Matthiessen, P.
(1986) Nine-headed dragon river. New York, NY: Shambhala.Google Scholar
Mayer, M.
(1967) A boy, a dog, and a frog. New York, NY: Dial Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Rollins, P. R.
(1994) Assessment of preschool narrative skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 3, 45–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Polunenko, A., & Marinellie, S. A.
(2009) Role of working memory in children’s understanding spoken narrative: A preliminary investigation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 485–509. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, S.
(2010) Birdology. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Munoz, M., Gillam, R., Pena, E., & Gulley-Faehnle, A.
(2003) Measures of language development in fictional narratives of latino children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 332–342. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E.
(1987) Some observations from the perspective of the rare event cognitive comparison theory of language acquisition. In K. E. Nelson, & A. van Kleeck (Eds.), Children’s Language, Vol. 6 (pp.289–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(1989) Strategies for first language teaching. In M. Rice, & R. Schiefelbusch (Eds.), The teachability of language (pp.263–310). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
(2000) Methods for stimulating and measuring lexical and syntactic advances: Why Fiffins and lobsters can tag along with other recast friends. In L. Menn, & N. B. Ratner (Eds.), Methods for studying language production (pp.115–148). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., & Arkenberg, M. E.
(2008) Language and reading development reflect dynamic mixes of learning conditions. In M. Moody, & E. R. Silliman (Eds.), Brain, behavior, and learning in language and reading disorders (pp.315–348). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Craven, P. L., Xuan, Y., & Arkenberg, M.
(2004) Acquiring art, spoken language, sign language, text, and other symbolic systems: Developmental and evolutionary observations from a dynamic systems perspective. In J. M. Lucariello, J. A. Hudson, R. Fivush, & P. J. Bauer (Eds.), The development of the mediated mind: sociocultural context and cognitive development, (pp.175–222). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Welsh, J., Camarata, S., & Tjus, T.
(2001) A rare event transactional dynamic model of tricky mix conditions contributing to language acquisition and varied communicative delays. In K. E. Nelson, A. Aksu-Koc, & C. E. Johnson (Eds.), Children’s Language, Vol. 11 (pp.158–197). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A., & Richner, E. S.
(2007) From actors to agents to persons: The development of character representation in young children’s narratives. Child Development, 78, 412–429. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oliver, M.
(1997) West wind. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G.
(2003) Assessing narrative comprehension in young children. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 36–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D.
(2007) Lost in translation: Methodological considerations in cross-cultural research. Child Development, 78, 1255–1264. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Gillam, R., Malek, M., Ruiz-Felter, R., Resendiz, M., Fiestas, C., & Sabel, T.
(2006) Dynamic assessment of school-age children’s narrative ability: An experimental investigation of classification accuracy. Journal of Speech, Language, Hearing Research, 49(5), 1037–1057. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A.
(1999) Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language 26(1), 49–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A.
(1994) A social interactionist account of developing decontextualized narrative skill. Developmental Psychology, 30, 937–948. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Price, J., Roberts, J., & Jackson, S.
(2006) Structural development of the fictional narratives of African American preschoolers. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 178–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Suggate, S., Long, J., & Schaughency, E.
(2010) Children’s oral narrative and reading skills in the first 3 years of reading instruction. Reading and Writing, 23(6), 627–644. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Renfrew, C.
(1977) The bus story: A test of continuous speech. Oxford, UK: C. E. Renfrew.Google Scholar
Sah, W.
(2013) Global and local connections in Mandarin-speaking children’s narratives: A developmental study based on the frog story. In A. McCabe, & C. Chang (Eds.), Chinese language narration: Culture, cognition, and emotion (pp.85–114). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, P., & Hayward, D.
(2010) Who does what to whom: Introduction of referents in children’s storytelling from pictures. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 459–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schneider, P., Hayward, D., & Dubé, R.
(2006) Storytelling from pictures using the Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument. Journal of Speech Pathology and Audiology, 30(4), 224–238.Google Scholar
Slackman, E., Hudson, J., & Fivush, R.
(1986) Actions, actors, links, and goals: The structure of children’s event representations. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure andfunction in development (pp.47–69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C.
(2007) Cross-cutting themes and future research directions. In D. August, & T. Shanahan (Eds.). Developing literacy in second-language learners, Report of the National Literacy Panel on language-minority children and youth (pp.631–651). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R.
(1997) Building complexity and coherence: Children’s use of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative development: Six approaches (pp.5–44). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stein, N., & Glenn, C.
(1979) An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp.53–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Strömqvist, S., & Verhoeven. L.
(Eds.) (2004) Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tabors, P. O., Snow, C. E., & Dickinson, D. K.
(2001) Homes and schools together: Supporting language and literacy development. In P. O. Tabors, & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school (pp.313–334). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B.
(2006) Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology (6th ed), Vol. 1 (pp.258–312). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M.
(1994) The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes, 15, 249–275. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., & Rodkin, P.
(1994) Knowledge of goal/plans: A conceptual basis for narrating Frog, where are you. In S. Strömqvist, & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: Typological and contextual perspectives (pp.85–106). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Troseth, G. L., Bloom, M., & Deloache, J. S.
(2007) Young children’s use of scale models: Testing alternatives to representational insight. Developmental Science, 10, 763–769. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den Broek, P., Lorch, E. P., & Thurlow, R.
(1996) Children’s and adult’s memory for television stories: The role of causal factors, story-grammar categories, and hierarchical level. Child Development, 67, 3010–3028. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., & Hudelot, C.
(2009) Explaining events in narratives: The impact of scaffolding in 4 to 12 old-year children. Psychology of Language and Communication, 13(1), 3–20. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E., Nir, B., & Hudelot, C.
(2014) Promoting children’s narrative skills through a short conversational intervention. Paper presented at the 13th Conference of the International Association for Studies of Child Language, Amsterdam, Netherlands.Google Scholar
Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K.
(2000) Sharing memories and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-year-olds. Memory, 8, 159–177. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Whitely, C., & Colozzo, P.
(2013) Who’s who? Memory updating and character reference in children’s narratives. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 1625–1636. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wong, A. M. Y., & Johnston, J. R.
(2004) The development of discourse referencing in Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 31, 633–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Xuan, Y.
(2006) Narrative Intervention on 4-year-old typically developing children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, University Park.Google Scholar
Zevenbergen, A. A., & Whitehurst, G. J.
(2003) Dialogic reading: A shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In A. van Kleek, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and teachers (pp.177–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 4 other publications

Kornev, Aleksandr N., Ingrida Balčiūnienė & Anastasia A. Vishonkina
2023. Acquisition Specifics of Genre Repertoire of Personal Discourse in 4-6-Year-Old Children with Developmental Language Disorder and Typically Developing Peers. RUDN Journal of Language Studies, Semiotics and Semantics 14:4  pp. 1067 ff. DOI logo
Veneziano, Edy
2019. Conversationally and Monologically-Produced Narratives: A Complex Story of Horizontal Décalages. Psychology of Language and Communication 23:1  pp. 85 ff. DOI logo
Veneziano, Edy & Eleonora Bartoli
2022. Individual differences in the effectiveness of a narrative-promoting intervention: Relation with executive function skills. First Language 42:4  pp. 523 ff. DOI logo
Veneziano, Edy, Marie-Thérèse Le Normand, Marie-Helène Plumet & Juliette Elie-Deschamps
2020. Promoting narrative skills in 5- to 8-year-old French-speaking children: The effects of a short conversational intervention. First Language 40:3  pp. 225 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 20 march 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.