References

References

Aksu-Koç, A
1998The role of input vs. universal predispositions in the emergence of tense-aspect morphology: Evidence from Turkish. First Language 18: 255–280. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, B., Pine, J. & Lieven, E
2014Child language acquisition: Why universal grammar doesn't help. Language 90(3): e53–e90. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Arad, M
2005Roots and Patterns. Hebrew Morpho-syntax. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S. & Berman, R. A
2003The emergence of grammar: Early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language 30: 845–877. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, M
2003Lexical Categories. Verbs, Nouns, and Adjectives [Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 102]. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bassano, D
2000Early development of nouns and verbs in French: Exploring the interface between lexicon and grammar. Journal of Child Language 27: 521–559. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bauer, L. & Varela, S
(eds) 2005Approaches to Conversion/Zero derivation. Münster: Waxmann.Google Scholar
Becquey, C
2014Diasystème, diachronie: Etudes comparées dans les langues cholanes. Utrecht: LOT (Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics). [URL] Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Armon-Lotem, S
1996How grammatical are early verbs? In Actes du Colloque International sur L’acquisition de la Syntaxe en Langue Maternelle et en Langue Étrangère, C. Martinot (ed.), 17–59. Paris: Les Belles Lettres.Google Scholar
Bernal, S., Lidz, J., Millotte, S. & Christophe, A
2007Syntax constrains the acquisition of verb meaning. Language Learning and Development 3: 325–341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bisang, W
2008Precategoriality and argument structure. In Archaic Chinese, Constructional Reorganization [Constructional Approaches to Languages 5], J. Leino (ed.), 55–88. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L
1971Why not pivot grammar? Journal of Speech and Disorder 36: 40–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1973One Word at a Time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. & Harner, L.
1989On the developmental contour of child language: A reply to Smith & Weist. Journal of Child Language 16: 207–215. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bottari, P., Cipriani, P. & Chilosi, A. M
1993–1994Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics 3: 327–369. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S
1963The ontogeny of English phrase structure: The first phase. Language 39: 1–13. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1976Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41(1, serial no. 164). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, R
1973A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brusini, P., de Carvalho, A., Dautriche, I., Gutman, A., Cauvet, E., Millotte, S., Amsili, P. & Christophe, A
To appear. Bootstrapping lexical and syntactic acquisition. In Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, Contexts, and Learners, M. Hickmann, E. Veneziano & H. Jisa (eds) Amsterdam John Benjamins
Carter, R
2006Polycategoriality and predictability: Problems and prospects. In Lexical Categories and Root Classes in Amerindian Languages, X. Lois & V. Vapnarsky (eds), 343–390. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
2010What have we learned, and what do we need to do to learn more about lexical categories? Paper presented at the International Conference on Polycategoriality “How Flexible are Lexical Categories ?” , 4–6 October, ENS, Paris.
Chung, S
2012Are lexical categories universal? The view from Chamorro. Theoretical Linguistics 38(1–2): 1–56. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V
1987The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In Mechanisms of language acquisition, B. MacWhinney (ed), 1–33. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cohen, M
1969Sur l'étude du langage enfantin. Enfance 3(3–4): 181–249.Google Scholar
1969Sur l'étude du langage enfantin. Enfance 22(3–4): 203–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Croft, W
1991Syntactic Categories and Grammatical Relations: The Cognitive Organization of Information. Chicago IL: University Press of Chicago.Google Scholar
2000Parts of speech as typological universals and language particular categories. In Approaches to the Typology of Word Classes, P. M. Vogel & B. Comrie (eds), 65–102. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2005Word classes, parts of speech and syntactic argumentation (Commentary on Evans and Osada, “Mundari: The myth of a language without word classes”). Linguistic Typology 9: 431–41.Google Scholar
Croft, W. & van Lier, E
2012Language universal without universal categories. Theoretical Linguistics 38(1–2): 57–72.Google Scholar
Danziger, E
2008A person, a place, or a thing? Whorfian consequences of syntactic bootstrapping in Mopan Maya. In Crosslinguistic Perspectives on Argument Structure, M. Bowerman & P. Brown (eds), 29–48. New York NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Demuth, K
2001A prosodic approach to filler syllables. Journal of Child Language 28: 246–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Don, J
2005On conversion, relisting and zero derivation. SKASE 2(2): 2–16. [URL] Google Scholar
Embick, D
2010Localism Versus Globalism in Morphology and Phonology. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Evans, N. & Osada, T
2005Mundari: The myth of a language without word classes. Linguistic Typology 9(3): 351–390.Google Scholar
Franchetto, B
2006Are Kuikuro roots lexical categories? In Lexical Categories and Root Classes in Amerindian Languages, X. Lois & V. Vapnarsky (eds), 33–68. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
2010The ergativity effect in Kuikuro (Southern Carib, Brazil). In Ergativity in Amazonia [Typological Studies in Language 89], S. Gildea & F. Queixalós (eds), 121–158. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Sebastian, E. & Soto, P
1999The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: Across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge? International Journal of Bilingualism 3: 133–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerken, L. & McIntosh, B
1993Interplay of function morphemes and prosody in early language. Developmental Psychology 29(3): 448–457. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gildea, S. O
1998On Reconstructing Grammar: Comparative Cariban Morphosyntax. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R
1990The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1: 3–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gold, E. M
1967Language identification in the limit. Information and Control 10: 447–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Grimshaw, J
1981Form, function, and the language acquisition device. In The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition, C. L. Baker & J. J. McCarthy (eds), 165–182. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grégoire, A
1937L’apprentissage du langage. Vol. 1. Paris: Librairie Droz.Google Scholar
1947L’apprentissage du langage. Vol. 2. Paris: Librairie Droz.Google Scholar
Guillaume, P
1927Le développement des éléments formels dans le langage de l’enfant. Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 24: 203–229.Google Scholar
Halle, M. & Marantz, A
1993Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The View from Building 20. Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, K. Hale & S.J. Keyser (eds), 111–176. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
1994Some key features of distributed morphology. In Papers on Phonology and Morphology [MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 21], A. Carnie & H. Harley (eds), 275–288. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M
2012Escaping ethnocentrism in the study of word-class universals. Theoretical Linguistics 38(1–2): 91–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hengeveld, K
1992Non-verbal Predication: Theory, Typology, Diachrony. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2008Prototypical and non-prototypical noun phrases in functional discourse grammar. In The Noun Phrase in Functional Discourse Grammar, D. García Velasco & J. Rijkhoff (eds), 43–62. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsburg, E. & Shatz, M
1982Linguistic input and the child's acquisition of language. Psychological Bulletin 92: 3–26. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Houis, M
1981Les schèmes d’énoncé en bambara. Mandenkan 1: 17–24.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Weissenborn, J., Kiefer, D., Schulz, A. & Schmitz, M
2004Functional elements in infants' speech processing: The role of determiners in the syntactic categorization of lexical elements. Infancy 5: 341–353. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Katz, N., Baker, E. & Macnamara, J
1974What’s in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. Child Development 45: 469–473. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kerleroux, F
1996La coupure invisible. Lille: Septentrion, Presses Universitaires de Lille.Google Scholar
2000Identification d?un procédé morphologique: La conversion. Faits de Langue 14: 89–100.Google Scholar
Küntay, A. & Slobin, D.I
2002Putting interaction back into child language: Examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication 6: 5–14.Google Scholar
Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Kilani-Schoch, M., Korecky-Kröll, K., Xanthos, A. & Dressler, W. U
2007Weakly inflecting languages: French, Dutch, and German. In Typological Perspectives on the Acquisition of Noun and Verb Morphology [Antwerp Papers in Linguistics 112], S. Laaha & S. Gillis (eds), 21–33. Antwerp: Antwerp University.Google Scholar
Launey, M
1994Une grammaire omniprédicative. Paris: CNRS-Editions.Google Scholar
Lehmann, C
2008Roots, stems and word classes. Studies in Languages 32(3): 546–567. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieber, R
2006The category of roots and the roots of categories: What we learn from selection in derivation. Morphology 16: 247–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E
2014First language development: A usage-based perspective on past and current research. Journal of Child Language 41: 48–63. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Lier, E
2012Reconstructing multifunctionality. Theoretical Linguistics 38(1–2): 119–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lléo, C
1997Filler syllables, proto-articles and early prosodic constraints in Spanish and German. In Language Acquisition: Knowledge, Representation, and Processing [Proceedings GALA], A. Sorace, C. Heycock & R. Shillcock (eds), 251–256. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Lois, X
2011Roots and patterns in Yucatecan languages. In Proceedings of Formal Approaches to Mayan Linguistics [MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 63], K. Shklovksy, P. Mateo Pedro & J. Coon (eds), 147–169. Cambridge MA: MITWPL.Google Scholar
Lois, X. & Vapnarsky, V
2006Introduction. In Lexical Categories and Root Classes in Amerindian Languages, X. Lois & V. Vapnarsky (eds), 1–30. Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Luuk, E
2009The noun/verb and predicate/argument structures. Lingua 119: 1707–1727. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
MacNamara, J
1982Names for Things. A Study of Human Learning. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marantz, A
1997No escape from syntax. In University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, A. Dimitriadis & I. Siegel (eds), 201–225. Philadelphia PA: University of Pennsylvania Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Miller, W. & Ervin, S
1964. The development of grammar in child language. In The Acquisition of Language [Child Development Monographs 29], U. Belugi & R. Brown (eds). Chicago IL: Chicago Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. M. & Menn, L
1993False starts and filler-syllables: Ways to learn grammatical morphemes. Language 69: 742–777. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S
1984Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E. & Caselli, M. C
1992The acquisition of Italian morphology: Implications for models of language development. Journal of Child Language 19: 491–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rijkhoff, J. & van Lier, E
(eds) 2013Flexible Word Classes: A Typological Study of Underspecified Parts-of-speech. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Robert, S
(ed) 2003Perspectives synchroniques sur la grammaticalisation. Polysémie, transcatégorialité et échelles syntaxiques. Louvain: Peeters.Google Scholar
Rumsey, A., San Roque, L., & Schieffelin, B
2013The acquisition of ergative marking in Kaluli, Ku Waru and Duna (Trans New Guinea). In The Acquisition of Ergativity, E.. Bavin and S. Stoll (eds), 133–182. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sadock, J. M
1999The nominalist theory of Eskimo: A case study of scientific self-deception. International Journal of American Linguistics 65(4): 383–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B
1985The acquisition of Kaluli. In The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition, Vol. 1, D. Slobin (ed.), 525–593. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
1990The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Scholz, B. C
2004Gold's theorems and the logical problem of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 31: 959–961. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R., Werker, J. & Cutler, A
2006Recognition and representation of function words in English-learning infants. Infancy 10: 187–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R. & Lepage, M
2008The effect of functional morphemes on word segmentation in pre-verbal infants. Developmental Science 11(3): 407–413. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Shi, R. & Melançon, A
2010Syntactic categorization in French-learning infants. Infancy 15: 517–533. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D.I
1973Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Studies of Child Language Development, C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (eds), 175–208. New York NY: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A
(eds) 1977Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M
1992First Verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2003Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Vapnarsky, V. & Haag, M
. (org) 2009Workshop ‘Typological Perspectives on Polycategoriality’ , ALT 8, July, Berkeley CA.
Vapnarsky, V. & Lois, X
2009Morphosyntactic and semantic issues on polycategoriality in Yucatecan Mayan languages. Talk presented at Workshop ‘Typological Perspectives on Polycategoriality’ , ALT 8, July, Berkeley CA.
Veneziano, E
2004The emergence of expressive options in early child language: A constructivist account. In Perspectives on language and language development: Essays in honor of Ruth A. Berman, D. Ravid & H. Bat-Zeev Shyldkrot (eds), 203–218. Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C
In press. Retrieving meaning from noun and verb grammatical contexts: Interindividual variation among 2- to 4-year-old French-speaking children. In Sources of Variation in First Language Acquisition: Languages, Contexts, and Learners, M. Hickmann, E. Veneziano & H. Jisa (eds) Amsterdam John Benjamins
Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H
2000The changing status of 'filler syllables' on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27: 461–500. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C
2011Retrieving the meaning of words from syntactic cues: A comprehension study. XIIIth International Conference on the Study of Child Language , Montreal, Canada.
Waxman, S. R., Lidz, J. L., Braun, I. E. & Lavin, T
2009Twenty four-month-old infants' interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Cognitive Psychology 59(1): 67–95. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K. & Culicover, P
1980Formal Principles of Language Acquisition. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar