Chapter 5
Factors behind variation in marking information structure
Contributions from Central Pomo
Prosody and morphosyntax are exploited in most languages for expressing information structure, but the contributions of each vary. Some of the factors underlying the both cross-linguistic similarities and the variation are differences between the two, in particular (i) their patterns of development through time and (ii) their categoriality. While prosody does not normally lose its pragmatic force with frequency of use, syntactic and morphological constructions do. While pitch, intensity, and rhythm, are matters of degree, syntactic and morphological constructions, such as particular orders or the presence of markers, are categorical. Prosodic patterns appear to be tied to cognition more directly, such as the packaging of information into intonation units and correlations between prosodic prominence and pragmatic strength. These are generally more widespread. Syntactic and morphological patterns, the result of development over time, show more cross-linguistic variation. Interactions between the two are illustrated with examples from spontaneous speech in Central Pomo, a language indigenous to California. The patterns include the packaging of information into intonation units, the expression of different kinds of topics (given and continuing, continuing but reconfirmed, shifted, contrastive), and different kinds of focus (broad, narrow of various types, contrastive, corrective, exhaustive, and additive).
Article outline
- 1.Introduction
- 2.Central Pomo
- 3.Givenness and basic prosodic packaging
- 4.Topic constructions
- 4.1No mention: Continuing given topics
- 4.2Unstressed pronouns: Given referents
- 4.3Antitopics: Continuing topics
- 4.4Topicalization: Shift to accessible topic
- 4.5Separate clause or sentence: Brand new topic
- 4.6Topic spotlight clitic =ya
- 4.7Prosodically prominent, initial constituents: Contrastive topics
- 4.8Contrastive enclitic =na
- 4.9Passive: Elimination of non-topical agents
- 5.Focus constructions
- 5.1Broad (all new) focus
- 5.2In-situ narrow focus
- 5.3Initial-position focus
- 5.4Initial position contrastive focus
- 5.5Clefts
- 5.6Exhaustive focus
- 5.7Additive focus
- 6.Strategies for marking information structure
- 6.1Activation state
- 6.2Topic
- 6.3Focus
- 6.4Prosody, syntax, and morphology
-
References
References (23)
References
Asiatani, Rusudan & Skopeteas, Stavros. 2013. The information structure of Georgian. In Krifka & Musan (eds), 127–158.
Chafe, Wallace. 1976. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In Subject and Topic, Charles Li (ed.), 25–50. New York NY: Academic Press.
Chafe, Wallace. 1979. The flow of thought and the flow of language. In Discourse and Syntax, Talmy Givón, (ed.), 159–181. New York NY: Academic Press.
Chafe, Wallace. 1980. The deployment of consciousness in the production of a narrative. In The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production, Wallace Chafe (ed.), 9–50. Norwood NJ: Ablex.
Chafe, Wallace. 1994. Discourse, Consciousness, and Time. Chicago IL: University of Chicago Press.
Evans, Nicholas & Levinson, Steven. 2009. The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 429–492.
Féry, Caroline. 2013. Focus as prosodic alignment. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 31: 683–734.
Fiedler, Ines & Schwarz, Anne. 2010. Introduction. In The Expression of Information Structure: A Documentation of its Diversity across Africa [Typological Studies in Language 91], Ines Fiedler & Anne Schwarz (eds), vii–xii. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Hedberg, Nancy. 2000. The referential status of clefts. Language 76(4): 891–920.
Klein, Wolfgang. 2013. The information structure of French. In Krifka & Musan (eds), 95–126.
Krifka, Manfred & Musan, Renate. 2012. Introduction. In Krifka & Musan (eds), 1–45.
Krifka, Manfred & Musan, Renate (eds). 2012. The Expression of Information Structure [The Expression of Cognitive Categories 5]. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Lambrecht, Knud. 1994. Information Structure and Sentence Form: Topic, Focus, and the Mental Representations of Discourse Referents. Cambridge: CUP.
Lambrecht, Knud. 2001. A framework for the analysis of cleft constructions. Linguistics 39: 463–516.
Lehmann, Christian. 2008. Information structure and grammaticalisation. In Theoretical and Empirical Issues in Grammaticalization [Typological Studies in Language 77], Elena Seoane & Maria José Lopez-Couso (eds), 207–230. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Meillet, Antoine. 1912. L’évolution des formes grammaticales. Scientia: rivista internazionale di sintesi scientifica 12: 384–400. Reprinted in Meillet, Antoine. 1921. Linguistique historique et linguistique générale, 130–148. Paris: Champion.
Prince, Ellen. 1981. Toward a taxonomy of given-new information. In Radical Pragmatics, Peter Cole (ed.), 223–256. New York NY: Academic Press.
Rooth, Mats. 1985. Association with Focus. PhD dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.
Rooth, Mats. 1992. A theory of focus interpretation. Natural Language Semantics 1: 75–116.
Skopeteas, Stavros. 2012. The empirical investigation of information structure. In Krifka & Musan (eds), 217–248.
Skopeteas, Stavros & Fanselow, Gisbert. 2010. Focus in Georgian and the expression of contrast. Lingua 120: 1370–1391.
van der Wal, Jenneke. 2015. Information structure, (inter)subjectivity and objectification. Journal of Linguistics 51(2): 425–464.
Cited by (2)
Cited by two other publications
Modicom, Pierre-Yves & Olivier Duplâtre
[no author supplied]
2023. ,
This list is based on CrossRef data as of 26 september 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.