Part of
Building Categories in Interaction: Linguistic resources at work
Edited by Caterina Mauri, Ilaria Fiorentini and Eugenio Goria
[Studies in Language Companion Series 220] 2021
► pp. 3572
References (107)
References
Anderson, Michael L. 2010. Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 245–266. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, Michel L. 2016. Précis of after phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39: 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Aydede, Murat & Robbins, Philip. 2009. The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
Baerger, Dana Royce & McAdams, Dan P. 1999. Life story coherence and its relation to psychological well-being. Narrative Inquiry 9: 69–96. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barotto, Allessandra & Mauri, Caterina. 2018. Constructing lists to construct categories. Italian Journal of Linguistics 30: 95–134.
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1982. Context-independent and context-dependent information in concepts. Memory & Cognition 10: 82–93. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1983. Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition 11: 211–227. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1985. Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11: 629–654.Google Scholar
1987. The instability of graded structure: Implications for the nature of concepts. In Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization, Ulric Neisser (ed.), 101–140. Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
1989. Intraconcept similarity and its implications for interconcept similarity. In Similarity and Analogical Reasoning, Stella Vosniadou & Andrew Ortony (eds), 76–121. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1990. On the indistinguishability of exemplar memory and abstraction in category representation. In Content and Process Specificity in the Effects of Prior Experiences: Advances in Social Cognition [Advances in Social Cognition 3], Thomas K. Srull & Robert S. Wyer Jr. (eds), 61–88. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
1991. Deriving categories to achieve goals. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Advances in Research and Technology, Vol. 27, Gordon H. Bower (ed.), 1–64. New York NY: Academic Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
1992. Frames, concepts, and conceptual fields. In Frames, Fields, and Contrasts: New Essays in Semantic and Lexical Organization, Adrienne Lehrer & Eva F. Kittay (eds), 21–74. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
1993. Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In Theories of Memory, Alan F. Collins, Susan E. Gathercole, Martin A. Conway & Peter E. Morris (eds), 29–101. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 577–660. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2003. Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes 18: 513–562. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–645. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2009. Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364: 1281–1289. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2012. The human conceptual system. In The Cambridge Handbook of Psycholinguistics, Michael Spivey, Ken McRae & Marc F. Joanisse (eds), 239–258. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016a. Can cognition be reduced to action? Processes that mediate stimuli and responses make human action possible. In Where’s the Action? The Pragmatic Turn in Cognitive Science [Strüngmann Forum Reports 18], Andreas K. Engel, Karl J. Friston & Danica Kragic (eds), 81–96. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016b. On staying grounded and avoiding quixotic dead ends. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 1122–1142. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016c. Situated conceptualization offers a theoretical account of social priming. Current Opinion in Psychology 12: 6–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2016d. Situated conceptualization: Theory and applications. In Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment [Foundations of Embodied Cognition 1], Yann Coello & Martin H. Fischer (eds), 11–37. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
2017. Cognitively plausible theories of concept composition. In Compositionality and Concepts in Linguistics and Psychology, James A. Hampton & Yoad Winter (eds), 9–30. Dordrecht: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., & Sewell, D. R. (1984). Constructing representations of categories from different points of view. Emory Cognition Project Technical Report #2, Emory University.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W., Breazeal, Cynthia & Smith, Linda B. 2007. Cognition as coordinated non-cognition. Cognitive Processing 8: 79–91. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. & Hale, Christopher R. 1993. Components of conceptual representation. From feature lists to recursive frames. In Categories and Concepts: Theoretical Views and Inductive Data Analysis, Ivan Van Mechelen, James A. Hampton, Ryszard Michalski & Peter Theuns (eds), 97–144. San Diego CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W., Niedenthal, Paula M., Barbey, Aron K. & Ruppert, Jennifer A. 2003. Social embodiment. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 43, Brian H. Ross, 43–92. New York NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W., Dutriaux Léo & Scheepers, Christoph. 2018. Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373: 20170144. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Borkenau, Peter. 1990. Traits as ideal-based and goal-derived social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58: 381–396. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bouton, Mark E. & Todd, Travis P. 2014. A fundamental role for context in instrumental learning and extinction. Behavioural Processes 104: 13–19. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brooks, Rodney A. 1991. Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence 47: 139–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carey, Susan. 2009. The Origin of Concepts. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, Shelly & Trope, Yaacov. 1999. Dual-process Theories in Social Psychology. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chaplin, William F., John, Oliver P. & Goldberg, Lewis R. 1988. Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 541–557. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chen, Jing, Papies, Esther K. & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2016. A core eating network and its modulations underlie diverse eating phenomena. Brain and Cognition 110: 20–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Chrysikou, Evangelia G. 2006. When shoes become hammers: Goal-derived categorization training enhances problem-solving performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32: 935–942.Google Scholar
Clark, Andy. 1998. Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press, A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
. 2008. Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coello, Yann & Fischer, Martin H. (eds). 2016a. Perceptual and Emotional Embodiment [Foundations of Embodied Cognition 1]. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
(eds). 2016b. Conceptual and Interactive Embodiment [Foundations of Embodied Cognition 2]. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
De Vega, Manuel, Glenberg, Arthur M. & Graesser, Arthur C. (eds). 2008. Symbols, Embodiment, and Meaning. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Domjan, Michael. 2014. The Principles of Learning and Behavior. Independence KY: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Donald, Merlin. 1993. Precis of Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 737–748. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Edson Escalas, Jennifer. 2004. Narrative processing: Building consumer connections to brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology 14: 168–180. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1985. Frames and the semantics of understanding. Quaderni Di Semantica 6: 222–254.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. 1975. The Language of Thought. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry A. & Pylyshyn, Zenon W. 1988. Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition 28: 3–71. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James J. 1966. The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems. Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
1979. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. 1997. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20: 41–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Goldstone, Robert L., Kersten, Alan & Carvalho, Paulo F. 2018. Categorization and concepts. In Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, John T. Wixted (ed.), 1–43. New York NY: Wiley & Sons. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hampton, James A. 1979. Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 441–461. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hinzen, Wolfram, Machery, Edouard & Werning, Markus (eds). 2012. The Oxford Handbook of Compositionality. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoenig, Klaus, Müller, Cornelia, Herrnberger, Bärbel, Sim, Eun-Jin, Spitzer, Manfred, Ehret, Günter & Kiefer, Markus. 2011. Neuroplasticity of semantic representations for musical instruments in professional musicians. NeuroImage 56: 1714–1725. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hsu, Nina S., Frankland, Steven M., & Thompson-Schill, Sharon L. 2012. Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowledge. Neuropsychologia 50: 327–333. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Huettig, Falk, Rommers, Joost & Meyer, Antje S. 2011. Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica 137: 151–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hurley, Susan. 2001. Perception and action: Alternative views. Synthese 129: 3–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, Edwin. 1995. Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kiefer, Markus, Sim, Eun-Jin, Herrnberger, Bärbel, Grothe, Jo & Hoenig, Klaus. 2008. The sound of concepts: Four markers for a link between auditory and conceptual brain systems. The Journal of Neuroscience 28: 12224–12230. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lebois, Lauren A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, Christine D., Simmons, W. Kyle, Barrett, Lisa F. & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2018. Learning situated emotions. Neuropsychologia 145: 106637. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lebois, Lauren A. M., Wilson-Mendenhall, Christine D. & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2015. Are automatic conceptual cores the gold standard of semantic processing? The context-dependence of spatial meaning in grounded congruency effects. Cognitive Science 39, 1764–1801. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lewis, James W. 2006. Cortical networks related to human use of tools. The Neuroscientist 12: 211–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
2010. Audio-visual perception of everyday natural objects – Hemodynamic studies in humans. In Multisensory Object Perception in the Primate Brain, Jochen Kaiser & Marcus Johannes Naumer (eds), 155–190. New York NY: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Loken, Barbara & Ward, James. 1990. Alternative approaches to understanding the determinants of typicality. Journal of Consumer Research 17: 111–126. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lynch, Elizabeth B., Coley, John D. & Medin, Douglas L. 2000. Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions, and graded category structure among tree experts and novices. Memory & Cognition 28: 41–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Malt, Barbara C. 1995. Category coherence in cross-cultural perspective. Cognitive Psychology 29: 85–148. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Martin, Alex. 2016. GRAPES—Grounding representations in action, perception, and emotion systems: How object properties and categories are represented in the human brain. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 23: 979–990. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mauri, Caterina. 2017. Building and interpreting ad hoc categories: A linguistic analysis. In Formal Models in the Study of Language, Joanna Blochowiak, Cristina Grisot, Stephanie Durrleman-Tame & Christopher Laenzlinger (eds), 297–326. Berlin: Springer. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mauri, Caterina & Sansò, Andrea. 2018. Linguistic strategies for ad hoc categorization: Theoretical assessment and cross-linguistic variation. In Linguistic Strategies for the Construction of Ad Hoc Categories: Synchronic and Diachronic Perspectives, Caterina Mauri & Andrea Sansò (eds). Special issue of Folia Linguistica 52(s39-1): 1–35. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McRae, Ken & Jones, Michael N. 2013. Semantic memory. In The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology, Daniel Reisberg (ed.), 206–219. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Miller, George A., Galanter, Eugene & Pribram, Karl H. 1960. Plans and the Structure of Behavior. New York NY: Holt Reinhart & Winston. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murphy, Gregory L. 2002. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Murphy, Gregory & Medin, Douglas L. 1985. The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review 92: 289–316. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Newell, Alan & Simon, Herbert A. 1972. Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Newen, Albert, Bruin, Leon D. & Gallagher, Shaun (eds). 2018. The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford: OUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, Paula M., Barsalou, Lawrence W., Winkielman, Piotr, Krauth-Gruber, Silvia & Ric, François. 2005. Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review 9: 184–211. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Papies, Esther K. & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2015. Grounding desire and motivated behavior: A theoretical framework and review of empirical evidence. In The Psychology of Desire, Wilhelm Hofmann & Loran F. Nordgren (eds), 36–60. New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pecher, Diane & Zwaan, Rolf A. (eds). 2005. Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, Friedemann. 1999. Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 253–279. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2005. Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 576–582. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, Friedeman, Moseley, Rachel L., Egorova, Natalia, Shebani, Zubaida & Boulenger, Véronique. 2014. Motor cognition–motor semantics: Action perception theory of cognition and communication. Neuropsychologia 55: 71–84. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Zenon W. 1984. Computation and Cognition. Toward a Foundation of Cognitive Science. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press, A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
Read, Stephen J., Jones, David K. & Miller, Lynn C. 1990. Traits as goal-based categories: The importance of goals in the coherence of dispositional categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58: 1048–1061. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Reese, Elaine, Haden, Catherine A., Baker-Ward, Lynne, Bauer, Patricia, Fivush, Robyn & Ornstein, Peter A. 2011. Coherence of personal narratives across the lifespan: A multidimensional model and coding method. Journal of Cognition and Development 12: 424–462. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor & Mervis, Carolyn B. 1975. Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology 7: 573–605. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor, Mervis, Carolyn B., Gray, Wayne D., Johnson, David M. & Boyes-Braem, Penny. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8: 382–439. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sherman, Jeffrey W., Gawronski, Bertram & Trope, Yaacov. 2014. Dual-process Theories of the Social Mind. New York NY: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Shtyrov, Yury, Butorina, Anna, Nikolaeva, Anastasia & Stroganova, Tatiana. 2014. Automatic ultrarapid activation and inhibition of cortical motor systems in spoken word comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences E1918–E1923. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simmons, W. Kyle, Martin, Alex & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2005. Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cerebral Cortex 15: 1602–1608. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Simmons, W. Kyle, Ramjee, Vimal, Beauchamp, Michael S., McRae, Ken, Martin, Alex & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2007. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia 45: 2802–2810. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, Edward E. & Medin, Douglas L. 1981. Categories and Concepts. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stein, Nancy L., & Hernandez, Marc W. 2007. Assessing understanding and appraisal during emotional experience. In Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, Jim A. Coan & John J. B. Allen (eds), 298–317. Oxford: OUP.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, Michael K., Spivey-Knowlton, Michael J., Eberhard, Kathleen M. & Sedivy, Julie C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268: 1632–1634. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. 2009. The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Kruger, Anne C. & Ratner, Hilary H. 1993. Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 495–511. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Trumpp, Natalie M., Kliese, Daniel, Hoenig, Klaus, Haarmeier, Thomas & Kiefer, Markus. 2013. Losing the sound of concepts: Damage to auditory association cortex impairs the processing of sound-related concepts. Cortex 49: 474–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vallee-Tourangeau, Frederic, Anthony, Susan H. & Austin, Neville G. 1998. Strategies for generating multiple instances of common and ad hoc categories. Memory 6: 555–592. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van der Laan, Laura Nynke, de Ridder, Denise, T. D., Viergever, Max A. & Smeets, Paul A. M. 2011. The first taste is always with the eyes: A meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. NeuroImage 55: 296–303. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voorspoels, Wouter, Storms, Gert & Vanpaemel, Wolf. 2013. Idealness and similarity in goal-derived categories: A computational examination. Memory & Cognition 41: 312–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Voorspoels, Wouter, Vanpaemel, Wolf & Storms, Gert. 2010. Ideal representations in a similarity space. COGNITION IN FLUX 32: 2290–2295. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Voorspoels, Wouter, Vanpaemel, Wolf, & Storms, Gert. 2011. A formal ideal-based account of typicality. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18: 1006–1014. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wang, Xiaoying, Han, Zaizhu, He, Yong, Caramazza, Afonso, Song, Luping & Bi, Yanchao. 2013. Where color rests: Spontaneous brain activity of bilateral fusiform and lingual regions predicts object color knowledge performance. NeuroImage 76: 252–263. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Winter, Yoad & Hampton, James A. (eds). 2017. Compositionality and concepts in Linguistics and Psychology. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Wu, Ling-ling & Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2009. Perceptual simulation in conceptual combination: Evidence from property generation. Acta Psychologica 132: 173–189. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yao, Bo, Belin, Pascal & Scheepers, Christoph. 2011. Silent reading of direct versus indirect speech activates voice-selective areas in the auditory cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23: 3146–3152. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
. 2012. Brain ‘talks over’ boring quotes: Top-down activation of voice-selective areas while listening to monotonous direct speech quotations. NeuroImage 60: 1832–1842. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by (6)

Cited by six other publications

Beysembaeva, G.
2024. FRAME MODELLING OF THE SUBLANGUAGE OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING (IN RUSSIAN, GERMAN, FRENCH). Bulletin of Shokan Ualikhanov Kokshetau University Philological Series :3  pp. 54 ff. DOI logo
Talanquer, Vicente & Resa Kelly
2024. Thinking and Learning in Nested Systems: The Individual Level. Journal of Chemical Education 101:2  pp. 283 ff. DOI logo
Martens, Betsy Van der Veer
2023. Representation, Reference, Relevance, and Retention. In Keywords In and Out of Context [Synthesis Lectures on Information Concepts, Retrieval, and Services, ],  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Turner, Phil
2023. Creating a Good Experience. In A Psychology of User Experience [Human–Computer Interaction Series, ],  pp. 25 ff. DOI logo
Barotto, Alessandra & Caterina Mauri
2022. Non-exhaustive connectives. STUF - Language Typology and Universals 75:2  pp. 317 ff. DOI logo
Dove, Guy, Laura Barca, Luca Tummolini & Anna M. Borghi
2022. Words have a weight: language as a source of inner grounding and flexibility in abstract concepts. Psychological Research 86:8  pp. 2451 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 5 november 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.