Article published In:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 7:2 (2017) ► pp.203231
References (65)
References
Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 191, 10611–070.Google Scholar
Allbritton, D. W., & Gerrig, R. J. (1991). Participatory responses in text understanding. Journal of Memory & Language, 301, 603–626. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bohn-Gettler, C. M., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Depending on my mood: Mood-driven influences on text comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1031, 562–577. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014). Emotions in reading and writing. In R. Pekrun & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 437–457). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F. (1996). The nature of narrative suspense and the problem of rereading. In P. Vorderer, H. J. Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Suspense: Conceptualizations, theoretical analyses, and empirical explorations (pp. 107–127). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F., & Lichtenstein, E. H. (1982). Stories are to entertain: A structural-affect theory of stories. Journal of Pragmatics, 61, 473–486. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Carretti, B., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Romanò, M. (2005). Updating in working memory: A comparison of good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 911, 45–66. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cupchik, G. C., Leonard, G., Axelrad, E., & Kalin, J. D. (1998). The landscape of emotion in literary encounters. Cognition and Emotion, 121, 825–847. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2005). Why does rereading improve metacomprehension accuracy? Evaluating the levels-of-disruption hypothesis for the rereading effect. Discourse Processes, 401, 37–55. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Egidi, G., & Gerrig, R. J. (2009). How valence affects language processing: Negativity bias and mood congruence in narrative comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 371, 547–555. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J., Bagelmann, K. A., & Mumper, M. L. (2016). On the origins of readers’ outcome preferences. Discourse Processes, 531, 603–631. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gillioz, C., Gygax, P., & Tapiero, I. (2012). Individual differences and emotional inferences during reading comprehension. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 661, 239–250. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 791, 701–721. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gygax, P., Tapiero, I., & Carruzzo, E. (2007). Emotion inferences during reading comprehension: What evidence can the self-pace reading paradigm provide? Discourse Processes, 441, 33–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Horton, W. S., & Rapp, D. N. (2003). Out of sight, out of mind: Occlusion and the accessibility of information in narrative comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 101, 104–110. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoshi, S. (1985). “The cost of kindness” and other fabulous tales. Philadelphia, PA: C and P Press.Google Scholar
Ishikawa, T. (1992). Aibiki. Kodansha. Tokyo. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
Jacovina, M. E., Hinze, S. R., & Rapp, D. N. (2014). Fool me twice: The consequences of reading (and rereading) inaccurate information. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 281, 558–568 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Keenan, J. M. (1973). Reading rate as a function of the number of propositions in the base structure of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 257–274. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., Kozminsky, E., Streby, W. J., McKoon, G., & Keenan, J. M. (1975). Comprehension and recall oftext as a function of content variables. Journal ofVerbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 141, 196–214. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Komeda, H., Kawasaki, M., Tsunemi, K., & Kusumi, T. (2009). Differences between estimating protagonists’ emotions and evaluating readers’ emotions in narrative comprehension. Cognition and Emotion, 231, 135–151. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Komeda, H., & Kusumi, T. (2006). The effect of a protagonist’s emotional shift on situation model construction. Memory & Cognition, 341, 1548–1556. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Komeda, H., Tsunemi, K., Inohara, K., Kusumi, T., & Rapp, D. N. (2013). Beyond disposition: The processing consequences of explicit and implicit invocations of empathy. Acta Psychologica, 1421, 349–355. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Komeda, H., Nihei, Y., & Kusumi, T. (2005). The roles of readers’ feelings in understanding narratives: Forefeel, empathy, and a sense of strangeness. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 751, 479–486. (In Japanese with English abstract). DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 161, 149–157.Google Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Skowronski, J. J., Britt, M. A., Guss, C. D., & Forsythe, C. (2008). What do you want? How perceivers use cues to make goal inferences about others. Cognition, 1061, 594–632. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Magliano, J. P., Taylor, H. A., & Kim, H. J. (2005). When goals collide: Monitoring the goals of multiple characters. Memory & Cognition, 331, 1357–1367. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 31, 173–192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McCrudden, M. T., Schraw, G., & Kambe, G. (2005). The effect of relevance instructions on reading time and learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 971, 88–102. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence. Discourse Processes, 221, 247–288. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mensink, M. C., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Evil geniuses: Inferences derived from evidence and preferences. Memory & Cognition, 391, 1103–1116. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Miall, D. S. (1989). Beyond the schema given: Affective comprehension of literary narratives. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 55–78. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). Literary discourse. In Graesser, A., Gernsbacher, M. A., & Goldman, S. R. (Eds.), Handbook of discourse processes. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 321–355.Google Scholar
Miall, D. S., & Kuiken, D. (1995). Aspects of literary response: A new questionnaire. Research in the Teaching of English, 291, 37–58.Google Scholar
(2001). Shifting perspectives: Readers’ feelings and literary response. In van Peer, W. & Chatman, S. (Eds.), New perspectives on narrative perspective (pp. 289–301). Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
(2002). A feeling for fiction: Becoming what we behold. Poetics, 301, 221–241. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Millis, K. K., Simon, S., & tenBroek, N. S. (1998). Resource allocation during the rereading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 261, 232–246. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K. (1994). A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics, 231, 53–74. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2002). Emotions and the story worlds of fiction. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact (pp. 39–69). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 29–50. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G. (1998). Updating a situation model: A memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 241, 1200–1210.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 971, 315–331. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Peshkam, A., Mensink, M. C., Putnam, A. L., & Rapp, D. N. (2011). Warning readers to avoid irrelevant information: When being vague might be valuable. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 361, 219–231. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pettijohn, K. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (2016). Narrative event boundaries, reading times, and expectation. Memory & Cognition, 441, 1064–1075. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Polichak, J. W., & Gerrig, R. J. (2002). “Get up and win!”: Participatory responses to narrative. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 71–95). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Radvansky, G. A., Zwaan, R. A., Curiel, J. M., & Copeland, D. E. (2001). Situation models and aging. Psychology and Aging, 161, 145–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Raney, G. E., Therriault, D., & Minkoff, S. (2000). Repetition effects from paraphrased text: Evidence for an integrated representation model of text representation. Discourse Processes, 291, 61–81. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, D. N. (2008). How do readers handle incorrect information during reading? Memory & Cognition, 361, 688–701. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, D. N., & Gerrig, R. J. (2002). Readers’ reality-driven and plot-driven analyses in narrative comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 301, 779–788. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006). Predilections for narrative outcomes: The impact of story contexts and reader preferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 541, 54–67. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, D. N., Gerrig, R. J., & Prentice, D. A. (2001). Readers’ trait-based models of characters in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 451, 737–750. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, D. N., Komeda, H., & Hinze, S. R. (2011). Vivifications of literary investigation. The Scientific Study of Literature, 11, 123–135. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Scrimin, S., & Mason, L. (2015). Does mood influence text processing and comprehension? Evidence from an eye-movement study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 851, 387–406. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van den Broek, P., & Kendeou, P. (in press). The development of language comprehension skills: Change and continuity in the ability to build coherence. In K. Cain & D. Compton (Eds.), Theories of reading development, a 20 year celebration of scientific studies of reading. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.
van den Broek, P., Young, M., Yuhtsuen, T., & Linderholm, T. (1999). The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the online construction of memory representation. In H. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
VandenBos, G. R. (Ed.). (2007). APA dictionary of psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Wallot, S., Hollis, G., & van Rooij, M. (2013). Connected text reading anddifferences in text reading fluency in adult readers. PLoS One, 81:e71914. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, D. (1994). Mechanisms of emotional involvement with drama. Poetics, 231, 33–51. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A. (1999a). Five dimensions of narrative comprehension: The event-indexing model. In S. R. Goldman, A. C. Graesser, & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Narrative comprehension, causality, and coherence: Essays in honor of Tom Trabasso (pp. 93–110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
(1999b). Embodied cognition, perceptual symbols, and situation models. Discourse Processes, 281, 81–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Magliano, J. P., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). Dimensions of situation model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 211, 386–397.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situational models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 1231, 162–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Radvansky, G. A., Hilliard, A. E., & Curiel, J. M. (1998). Constructing multidimensional situation models during reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21, 199–220. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Rapp, D. N. (2006). Discourse comprehension. In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 725–764). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. DOI logoGoogle Scholar