Article published In:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 9:1 (2019) ► pp.72103
References
Altmann, U., Bohrn, I., Lubrich, O., Menninghaus, W., & Jacobs, A.
(2012) The power of emotional valence-from cognitive to affective processes in reading. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 61, 192. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) Fact vs fiction-how paratextual information shapes our reading processes. Social, Cognitive, Affective Neuroscience, 91, 22–29. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bálint, K., & Tan, E.
(2015) It feels like there are hooks inside my chest: The construction of narrative absorption using image schemata. Projections, 91, 63–88. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bálint, K., Hakemulder, F., Kuijpers, M., Doicaru, M. & Tan, E.
(2016) Reconceptualizing foregrounding. Scientific Study of Literature. 61, 176–207. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Baker, N.
(2010) The anthologist. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Bal, P., & Veltkamp, M.
(2013) How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation on the role of emotional transportation. PLoS ONE, 81, e55341. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bergen, B.
(2012) Louder than words: The new science of how the mind makes meaning. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P.
(1979) Les trois etats du capital culturel. Acres Rech. Sci. Soc., 301, 3–6.Google Scholar
(1980) Le capital social: Notes provisoires. Acres Rech. Sci. Soc., 311, 2–3.Google Scholar
(1985) The forms of capital. In J. G. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and research for the cociology of education (pp. 241–258). New York, NY: Greenwood.Google Scholar
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P.
(2003) Psychonarratology: Foundations for the empirical study of literary response. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowdle, B., & Gentner, D.
(2005) The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 1121, 193–216. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burke, M., Kuzmičová, A., Mangen, A., & Schilhab, T.
(2016) Empathy at the confluence of neuroscience and empirical literary studies. Scientific Study of Literature 6(1), 6–41. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Caracciolo, M.
(2012) Narrative, meaning, interpretation: An enactivist approach. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 111, 367–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) Phenomenological metaphors in readers’ engagement with characters: The case of Ian McEwan’s Saturday , Language and Literature, 221, 60–76. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2014) The experientiality of narrative: An enactivist approach. Berlin, Germany: DeGruyter. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Claassen, E.
(2012) Author representations in literary reading. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.
(1996) Using language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Citron, F., & Goldberg, A.
(2014) Social context modulates the effect of hot temperature on perceived interpersonal warmth: a study of embodied metaphors. Language and Cognition, 61, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Colston, H.
(2015) Using figurative language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Desai, R., Binder, J., Conant, L., Mano, Q., & Seidenberg, M.
(2011) The neural career of sensory-motor metaphors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 231, 2376–2386. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Djikic, M., & Oatley, K.
(2014) The art in fiction: From indirect communication to changes of the self. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 81, 498–505. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dillard, A.
(1982) Living by fiction. New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Finke, R.
(1989) Principles of Mental Imagery. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
Fong, K., Mullin, J., & Mar, R.
(2015) How exposure to literary genres shapes attitudes toward gender roles and sexual behavior. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 91, 274–285. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Frost, R.
(1979) The poetry of Robert Frost: The collected poems, complete and unabridged. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S.
(2017) Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G.
(2005) The brain’s concepts” The role of sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 221, 455–479. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Geary, J.
(2012) I is an other: The secret life of metaphor and how it shapes the way we see the world. New York, NY: Harpers.Google Scholar
Gerrig, R., & Mumper, M.
(2017) How readers’ lives affect narrative experiences. In M. Burke & E. Troscianko (Eds.), Cognitive literary science: Dialogues between literature and cognition (pp. 239–257). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.
(1994) The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(1999) Intentions in the experience of meaning. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
(2016) Embodied dynamics in literary experience. In M. Burke & E. Troscianko (Eds.), Dialogues between literature and cognition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2017) Metaphor wars: Conceptual metaphor in human life. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) The embodied and discourse views of metaphor: Why these are not so different and how can they be brought closer together. In B. Hampe (Ed.), Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Blackwell, N.
Gibbs, R., & Boers, E.
(2005) Metaphoric processing of allegorical poetry. In Z. Maalej (Ed.), Metaphor and culture. Manouba, Tunisia: University of Manouba Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Colston, H.
(2012) Interpreting figurative meaning. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Gerrig, R.
(1989) How context makes metaphor comprehension seem special. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 41, 145–158. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Gould, J., & Andric, M.
(2005–2006) Imagining metaphorical actions: Embodied simulations make the impossible plausible. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 251, 221–238. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Kushner, J., & Mills, R.
(1991) Authorial intentions and metaphor comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 201, 11–30. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Okonski, L., & Tendahl, M.
(2011) Inferring pragmatic messages from metaphor. Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 71, 3–28. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Rasse, C.
in preparation). Is reading literature a metaphoric activity?
Glenberg, A., & Kaschak, M.
(2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 91, 558–565. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., & Buccino, G.
(2008) Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 611, 905–919. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gorsuch, G., & Taguchi, E.
(2010) Developing reading fluency and comprehension using repeated reading: Evidence from longitudinal student reports. Language Teaching Research, 141, 27–59. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T.
(1994) Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 1031, 371–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Heaney, S.
(2014) Selected poems 1988–2013. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Horton, W. S.
(2007) Metaphor and reader’s attribution of intimacy. Memory and Cognition, 351, 87–94. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Iser, W.
(1972) The reading process: A phenomenological approach. New Literary History, 31, 279–299. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, A.
(2017) Affective and aesthetic processes in literary reading: A neurocognitive poetics perspective. In M. Burke & E. Troscianko (Eds.), Cognitive literary science: Dialogues between literature and cognition (pp. 309–325). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D.
(2012) Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 521, 150–155. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kidd, D., & Castano, E.
(2013) Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 3421, 377–380. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2019) Reading literary fiction and theory of mind: Three preregistered replications and extensions of Kidd and Castano (2013). Social Psychological and Personality Science, DOI HYPERLINK “ [URL] DOI logoDOI logoGoogle Scholar
Koopman, E., & Hakemulder, F.
(2015) Effects of literature on empathy and self-reflection: A theoretical-empirical framework. Journal of Literary Theory, 91, 79–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kotovych, M., Dixon, P., Bortolussi, M., & Holden, M.
(2011) Textual determinants of a component of literary identification. Scientific Study of Literature, 11, 260–291. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kukkonen, K.
(2014) Presence and prediction: The embodied reader’s cascades of cognition. Style, 481, 367–384.Google Scholar
Kuiken, D., Campbell, P., & Sopčák, P.
(2012) The Experiencing questionnaire: Locating exceptional reading moments. Scientific Study of Literature, 21, 243–272. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kuiken, D., & Douglas, S.
in press). Identifying aspects of absorption that facilitate aesthetic response. In F. Hakemulder, M. Kuijpers, E. Tan, M. Doicaru, K. Balin Eds. The handbook of narrative absorption New York, NY Oxford University Press
Kuzmičová, A.
(2014) Literary narrative and mental imagery: A view from embodied cognition. Style, 481, 275–293.Google Scholar
Leggitt, J., & Gibbs, R.
(2000) Emotional reactions to verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 291, 1–24. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lodge, D.
(Ed.) (1988) Modern criticism and theory: A reader. London, United Kingdom: Longman.Google Scholar
Louwerse, M., Kuiken, D.
(2004) The effects of personal involvement in narrative discourse: Introduction. Discourse Processes, 381, 169–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahlberg, M., Conklin, K., Bisson, M.-J.
(2014) Reading Dicken’s characters: Textual patterns and their cognitive reality. Language and Literature, 231, 369–388. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A.
(2008) A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis & a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 1021, 59–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mak, M., & Willems, R.
(2019) Mental simulation during literary reading: Individual differences revealed with eye-tracking. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 341, 511–535. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R., Oatley, K., Djikic, M., & Mullin, J.
(2011) Emotion and narrative fiction: Interactive influences before, during, and after reading. Cognition & Emotion, 251, 818–833. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mar, R., Oatley, K., & Peterson, J.
(2009) Exploring the link between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences and examining outcomes. Communications, 341, 407–428. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Mazzocco, P., Green, M., Sasota, J. & Jones, N.
(2010) This story is not for everyone: Transportability and narrative persuasion. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 11, 361–368. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McEwan, I.
(2008) On Chesil beach. London, United Kingdom: Vintage.Google Scholar
(2016) Nutshell. London, United Kingdom: Vintage.Google Scholar
Nabokov, V.
(1990) The defense. New York, NY: Vintage.Google Scholar
Oatley, K.
(2011) Such Stuff as Dreams: The psychology of fiction. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Okonski, L., & Gibbs, R.
(2019) Diving into the wreck: Can people resist allegorical meaning? Journal of Pragmatics, 1411, 28–43. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Panero, M., Weisberg, D., Black, J., Goldstein, T., Barnes, J., Brownell, H., & Winner, E.
(2016) Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Pfaff, K., & Gibbs, R.
(1997) Authorial intentions in understanding satirical texts. Poetics, 251, 45–70. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Popova, Y.
(2015) Stories, meaning, and experience: Narrativity and enaction. New York, NY: Routledge. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, D.
(2008) X is a journey: Embodied simulation in metaphor interpretation. Metaphor and Symbol, 231, 174–199. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2017) Contextual activation of story simulation in metaphor comprehension. In B. Hampe, Ed., Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R., & Kreuz, R.
(1994) Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 51, 159–163. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Sikora, S., Kuiken, D., & Miall, D.
(2011) Expressive reading: A phenomenological study of readers’ experience of Coleridge’s Rime of the Ancient Mariner. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and Art, 51, 258–268. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Conklin, K., & van Heuven, W.
(2011) Seeing a phrase ‘time and again’ matters: The role of phrasal frequency in the processing of multiword sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 371, 776–784.Google Scholar
Troscianko, E.
(2013) Reading imaginatively: The imagination in cognitive science and cognitive literary studies. Journal of Literary Semantics, 421, 181–198. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
van Kuijk, I., Verkoeijen, P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A.
(2018) The effect of reading a short passage of literary fiction on theory of mind: A replication of Kidd and Castano (2013). Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wilson, N., & Gibbs, R.
(2007) Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science, 311, 721–731. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wojciechowski, H. & Gallese, V.
(2011) How stories make us feel: Toward an embodied narratology. California Italian Studies 21.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., Stanfield, R., & Yaxley, R.
(2002) Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 131, 168–171. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. & Taylor, L.
(2006) Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1351, 1–11. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 7 other publications

Eekhof, Lynn S., Kobie van Krieken & Roel M. Willems
2022. Reading about minds: The social-cognitive potential of narratives. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 29:5  pp. 1703 ff. DOI logo
Gibbs, Raymond W.
2023. What’s figurative about figurative language?. Lingua 287  pp. 103520 ff. DOI logo
Gibbs, Raymond W. & Carina Rasse
2022. Embodiment in the diversity of literary experience: a reply to Wolfgang Teubert (2021). Journal of Literary Semantics 51:1  pp. 55 ff. DOI logo
Gibbs, Raymond W., Patrawat Samermit & Christopher R. Karzmark
2023. The Varieties of Ironic Experience. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 60 ff. DOI logo
Klochek, Hryhorii & Mariia Foka
2022. The Interaction of Receptive Poetics and Psycholinguistics: On the Way to the Synergetic Effect. Pertanika Journal of Social Sciences and Humanities 30:3  pp. 1095 ff. DOI logo
Rasse, Carina & Raymond W. Gibbs
2021. Metaphorical thinking in our literary experiences of J.D. Salinger’s “The Catcher in the Rye”. Journal of Literary Semantics 50:1  pp. 3 ff. DOI logo
[no author supplied]
2023. The Scope of Irony. In The Cambridge Handbook of Irony and Thought,  pp. 15 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 14 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.