Article published in:
Scientific Study of Literature
Vol. 9:1 (2019) ► pp. 333
References

[ p. 31 ]References

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
(2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bortolussi, M., & Dixon, P.
(1996) The effects of formal literary training on literary reception. Poetics, 23(6), 471–487. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Carrol, G., Conklin, K., Guy, J., & Scott, R.
(2016) Processing punctuation and word changes in different editions of prose fiction. Scientific Study of Literature, 5(2), 200–228. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chou, Y. M., Polansky, A. M., & Mason, R. L.
(1998) Transforming non-normal data to normality in statistical process control. Journal of Quality Technology, 30(2), 133–141. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conklin, K., Pellicer-Sánchez, A., & Carrol, G.
(2018) An introduction to eye-tracking: A guide for applied linguistics research. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, D.
(2015) Making a point: The pernickety story of English punctuation. London, United Kingdom: St Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Dickens, C.
(1846) The adventures of Oliver Twist. London, United Kingdom: Bradbury and Evans.Google Scholar
(1867) The Charles Dickens edition. The adventures of Oliver Twist. London, United Kingdom: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Filik, R., & Barber, E.
(2011) Inner speech during silent reading reflects the reader’s regional accent. PloS one, 6(10), e25782. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R., McCarthy, K. S., & Burkett, C.
(2015) Interpretative inferences in literature. In E. O’Brien, A. Cook & R. Lorch (Eds.), Inferences during reading. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Jiménez, E. M.
(2017) Unconventional patterns in the experimental poetry of E. E. Cummings: A stylistic approach to punctuation marks. Language and Literature, 26(3), 191–212. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Guy, J., Scott, R., Conklin, K., & Carrol, G.
(2016) Challenges in editing late nineteenth- and early twentieth-century prose fiction: What is “editorial completeness”? English Literature in Transition, 59(4), 435–55. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Graves, B., & Frederiksen, C. H.
(1991) Literary expertise in the description of fictional narrative. Poetics, 20(1), 1–26. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hakemulder, F., & van Peer, W.
(2016) Empirical stylistics. In V. Sotirova (Ed.), The Bloomsbury companion to stylistics. London, United Kingdom: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K.
(1967) Notes on transitivity and theme in English, part 2. Journal of Linguistics, 3(2), 199–244. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hill, R., & Murray, W.
(2000) Commas and spaces: Effects of punctuation on eye movements and sentence parsing. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller, & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 565–590). Oxford, United Kingdom: Elsevier. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hirotani, M., Frazier, L., & Rayner, K.
(2006) Punctuation and intonation effects on clause and sentence wrap-up: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 425–443. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 32 ]
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., & Westfall, P.
(2008) Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 346–363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P.
(1990) Eye movements during repeated reading of a text. Acta Psychologica, 73(3), 259–280. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
James, H.
(1881) The portrait of a lady. London, United Kingdom: Macmillan.Google Scholar
(1908) The portrait of a lady (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Charles Scribner’s Sons. Vol. III of The Novels and Tales of Henry James.Google Scholar
Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J.
(2010) Task effects on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1561–1566. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S., Lee, M., Park, H., Chang, M.-S., & Kwak, H. W.
(2015) Effects of search intent on eye-movement patterns in a change detection task. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 8(2), 1–10. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levy, B., Di Persio, R., & Hollingshead, A.
(1992) Fluent rereading: Repetition, automaticity, and discrepancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 957–71. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levine, S., & Horton, W. S.
(2013) Using affective appraisal to help readers construct literary interpretations. Scientific Study of Literature, 3(1), 105–136. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, K. S.
(2015) Reading beyond the lines: A critical review of cognitive approaches to literary interpretation and comprehension. Scientific Study of Literature, 5(1), 99–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Norris, M.
(2015) Between you and me: Confessions of a comma queen. London, United Kingdom: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Peskin, J.
(1998) Constructing meaning when reading poetry. An expert-novice study. Cognition and Instruction, 16(3), 235–263. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team
(2017) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from: https://​www​.R​-project​.org/
Sanford, A. J., & Filik, R.
(2007) “They” as a gender-unspecified singular pronoun: Eye tracking reveals a processing cost. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(2), 171–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Schotter, E., Bicknell, K., Howard, I., Levy, R., & Rayner, K.
(2014) Task effects reveal cognitive flexibility responding to frequency and predictability: Evidence from eye movements in reading and proofreading. Cognition, 131(1), 1–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sopčák, P.
(2007) “Creation from nothing”: A foregrounding study of James Joyce’s drafts for Ulysses. Language and Literature, 16(2), 183–196. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tartakovsky, R.
(2009) E. E. Cummings’ parentheses: Punctuation as poetic device. Style, 43(2), 215–47. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M.
(1994) Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of memory and language, 33, 285–285. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vipond, D., & Hunt, R. A.
(1984) Point driven understanding: Pragmatic and cognitive dimensions of literary reading. Poetics, 13(3), 261–277. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
[ p. 33 ]
Vitu, F.
(1991) The influence of parafoveal preprocessing and linguistic context on the optimal landing position effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 50(1), 58–75. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ward, P., & Sturt, P.
(2007) Linguistic focus and memory: An eye movement study. Memory & Cognition, 35(1), 73–86. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zeitz, C. M.
(1994) Expert-novice differences in memory, abstraction, and reasoning in the domain of literature. Cognition and Instruction, 12(4), 277–312. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Parente, Fabio, Kathy Conklin, Josephine M Guy & Rebekah Scott
2021. The role of empirical methods in investigating readers’ constructions of authorial creativity in literary reading. Language and Literature: International Journal of Stylistics 30:1  pp. 21 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 21 april 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.