Part of
Theories of Reading Development
Edited by Kate Cain, Donald L. Compton and Rauno K. Parrila
[Studies in Written Language and Literacy 15] 2017
► pp. 932
References
Adlof, S. M., Catts, H. W., & Little, T. D.
(2006) Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing, 19, 933–958. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Adlof, S. M., & Perfetti, C. A.
(2011, July). Are integration processes the same in reading and listening? Paper presented at the 18th annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, St. Pete Beach, FL.
Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien, E. J.
(1993) Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 1061–1070. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Amsel, B. D., DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M.
(2015) Close, but no garlic: Perceptuomotor and event knowledge activation during language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 82, 118–132. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. C.
(1978) Schema-directed processes in language comprehension. In A. M. Lesgold, J. W. Pellegrino, S. D. Fokkema, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Cognitive psychology and instruction (pp. 67–82). New York, NY: Plenum. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S., & Hersch, J.
(2010) Lexical precision in skilled readers: Individual differences in masked neighbor priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 299–318 DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S., & Lo, S.
(2012) Not all skilled readers have cracked the code: Individual differences in masked form priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 152–163.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C.
(1932) Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boets, B., de Beeck, H. P. O., Vandermosten, M., Scott, S. K., Gillebert, C. R., Mantini, D., … & Ghesquière, P.
(2013) Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science, 342, 1251–1254. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bolger, D. J., Balass, M., Landen, E., & Perfetti, C. A.
(2008) Context variation and definitions in learning the meanings of words: An instance-based learning approach. Discourse Processes, 45, 122–159. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, W. E.
(2007) Speaking up for vocabulary reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 226–243. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brown, C., & Hagoort, P.
(1993) The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 34–44. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burkhardt, P.
(2007) The P600 reflects cost of new information in discourse memory. Neuroreport, 18, 1851–1854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K.
(2010) Reading development and difficulties: An introduction. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cain, K. E., Bryant, P. E., & Oakhill, J.
(2004) Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J.
(2006) Profiles of children with specific reading comprehension difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 683–696. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., Barnes, M. A., & Bryant, P. E.
(2001) Comprehension skill, inference-making ability, and their relation to knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 29, 850–859. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V., & Bryant, P. E.
(2000) Investigating the causes of reading comprehension failure: The comprehension-age match design. Reading and Writing, 12, 31–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K.
(2004) Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 671–681. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J.
(2001) DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cromley, J. G., & Azevedo, R.
(2007) Testing and refining the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 311–325. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cutting, L. E., Materek, A., Cole, C. A. S., Levine, T., & Mahone, E. M.
(2009) Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance. Annals of Dyslexia, 59, 34–54. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G.
(1998) Context updating and the P300. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 152–154. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C.
(1998) When reading is “readn” or somthn. Distinctiveness of phonological representations of lexical items in normal and disabled readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 149–153. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Elbro, C., & Jensen, M. N.
(2005) Quality of phonological representations, verbal learning, and phoneme awareness in dyslexic and normal readers. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 375–384. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W.
(1995) Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A.
(1990) Language comprehension as structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldman, S. R., & Varma, S.
(1995) CAPing the construction-integration model of discourse comprehension. In C. A. Weaver, S. Mannes, & C. R. Fletcher (Eds.), Discourse comprehension: Essays in honor of Walter Kintsch (pp. 337–358). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Graesser, A. C., & Kreuz, R. J.
(1993) A theory of inference generation during text comprehension. Discourse Processes, 16, 145–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T.
(1994) Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–395. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hagoort, P.
(2005) On Broca, brain, and binding: A new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 416–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2013) MUC (memory, unification, control) and beyond. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.Google Scholar
Haviland, S. E., & Clark, H. H.
(1974) What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512–521. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Helder, A., Stafura, J., Calloway, R., van den Broek, P., & Perfetti, C.
(July 2015) Centrality effects on word to text integration during reading: an ERP study. Presented at the 25th Annual Meeting of the Society for Text & Discourse, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Hersch, J., & Andrews, S.
(2012) Lexical quality and reading skill: Bottom-up and top- down contributions to sentence processing. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 240–262. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B.
(1990) The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127–160. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A.
(1992) A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.
(1988) The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: a construction- integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Mross, E. F.
(1985) Context effects in word identification. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 336–349. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Rawson, K. A.
(2005) Comprehension. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 209–226). Malden, MA: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D.
(2000) Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 463–470. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A.
(1980) Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203–205. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Language and Reading Research Consortium
(2015) Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50, 151–169. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Lau, E., Almeida, D., Hines, P. C., & Poeppel, D.
(2009) A lexical basis for N400 context effects: Evidence from MEG. Brain and Language, 111, 161–172. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R.
(1992) Inference during reading. Psychological Review, 99, 440–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
McNamara, D. S., & Magliano, J.
(2009) Toward a comprehensive model of comprehension. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 297–384). New York, NY: Elsevier Science. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Myers, J. L., & O’Brien, E. J.
(1998) Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse processes, 26, 131–157. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Nation, K.
(2005) Connections between language and reading in children with poor reading comprehension. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connection between language and reading disabilities (pp. 41–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E.
(2003) The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., & Albrecht, J. E.
(1992) Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 777–784. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Cook, A. E., & Guéraud, S.
(2010) Accessibility of outdated information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 979–991.Google Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., & Myers, J. L.
(1985) When comprehension difficulty improves memory for text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 12–21.Google Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Plewes, P. S., & Albrecht, J. E.
(1990) Antecedent retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 241–249. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
O’Brien, E. J., Rizzella, M. L., Albrecht, J. E., & Halleran, J. G.
(1998) Updating a situation model: A memory-based text processing view. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1200–1210.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A.
(1999) Comprehending written language: A blueprint of the reader. In C. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), The neurocognition of language (pp. 167–208). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
(2007) Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L.
(2002) The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 67–86). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Landi, N., & Oakhill, J.
(2005) The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 227–247). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J.
(2014) Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 22–37. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2015) Comprehending implicit meanings in text without making inferences. In E. O’Brien, A. Cook, & R. Lorch. (Eds.), Inferences during reading (pp. 1–18). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Wlotko, E. W., & Hart, L. A.
(2005) Word learning and individual differences in word learning reflected in event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1281–1292.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C., Yang, C. L., & Schmalhofer, F.
(2008) Comprehension skill and word-to- text integration processes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 303–318. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G. D., Mouzaki, A., & Simos, P. G.
(2007) Development of lexical mediation in the relation between reading comprehension and word reading skills in Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 165–197. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rapp, D. N., & Taylor, H. A.
(2004) Interactive dimensions in the construction of mental representations for text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 988–1001.Google Scholar
Shank, R. C., & Abelson, R.
(1977) Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G.
(1988) A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, 95, 385–408. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rugg, M. D., & Curran, T.
(2007) Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 251–257. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloerscheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S., & Allan, K.
(1998) Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature, 392, 595–598. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Singer, M., & Halldorson, M.
(1996) Constructing and validating motive bridging inferences. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 1–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stafura, J. Z., & Perfetti, C. A.
(2014) Word-to-text integration: Message level and lexical level influences in ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 64, 41–53. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stafura, J. Z., Rickles, B., & Perfetti, C. A.
(2015) ERP evidence for memory and predictive mechanisms in word-to-text integration. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 30, 1273–1290. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W.
(2012) The simple view of reading redux vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45, 453–466. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Daalen-Kapteijns, M. M., & Elshout-Mohr, M.
(1981) The acquisition of word meanings as a cognitive learning process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 386–399. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, P. W., Helder, A., & Van Leijenhorst, L.
(2013) Sensitivity to Structural Centrality: Developmental and individual differences in reading comprehension skills. In: M. A. Britt, S. R. Goldman & J-F Rouet (Eds.), Reading: From words to multiple texts (pp. 132–146). New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Van den Broek, P., Risden, K., Fletcher, C. R., Thurlow, R., Britton, B., & Graesser, A.
(1996) A “landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 165–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W.
(1983) Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Van Petten, C., & Luka, B. J.
(2012) Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 176–190. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M.
(2004) Special reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Verhoeven, L., & Van Leeuwe, J.
(2008) Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Yang, C. L., Perfetti, C. A., & Schmalhofer, F.
(2005) Less skilled comprehenders ERPs show sluggish word-to-text integration processes. Written Language & Literacy, 8, 157–181. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(2007) Event-related potential indicators of text integration across sentence boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 55–89.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A.
(2003) The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 35–62. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., Langston, M. C., & Graesser, A. C.
(1995) The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: An event-indexing model. Psychological Science, 6, 292–297. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Madden, C. J.
(2004) Updating situation models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 283–288.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A.
(1998) Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 23 other publications

Cabell, Sonia Q. & HyeJin Hwang
2020. Building Content Knowledge to Boost Comprehension in the Primary Grades. Reading Research Quarterly 55:S1 DOI logo
Cabell, Sonia Q. & Tricia A. Zucker
2024. Using Strive‐for‐Five Conversations to Strengthen Language Comprehension in Preschool through Grade One. The Reading Teacher 77:4  pp. 522 ff. DOI logo
Clanton Harpine, Elaine
2019. How Do You Build a Group Program Based on the Principles of Intrinsic Motivation? Developing an After-School Program to Meet the Needs of all Students: Eight Years of Development. In After-School Programming and Intrinsic Motivation,  pp. 49 ff. DOI logo
Crosson, Amy C., Margaret G. McKeown, Puiwa Lei, Hui Zhao, Xinyue Li, Kelly Patrick, Kathleen Brown & Yaqi Shen
2021. Morphological analysis skill and academic vocabulary knowledge are malleable through intervention and may contribute to reading comprehension for multilingual adolescents. Journal of Research in Reading 44:1  pp. 154 ff. DOI logo
Davis, Dennis S., Jill S. Jones & Courtney Samuelson
2021. Is It Time for a Hard Conversation about Cueing Systems and Word Reading in Teacher Education?. Reading & Writing Quarterly 37:4  pp. 301 ff. DOI logo
Douglas, Dan
2024.  Assessing Academic English for Higher Education Admissions Assessing Academic English for Higher Education Admissions , by Xi, Xiaoming and Norris, John M (Eds.) New York, Routledge, 2021, 0815350643, 234 pp., USD $41, $54.95 (paperback), ISBN: 978-0-8153-5064-4 . Language Assessment Quarterly 21:1  pp. 113 ff. DOI logo
Georgiou, George K., Ana Paula Alves Vieira, Kyriakoula M. Rothou, John R. Kirby, Andrea Antoniuk, Dalia Martinez & Kan Guo
2023. A Meta-analysis of Morphological Awareness Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading 27:3  pp. 253 ff. DOI logo
Glasswell, Kathryn, Christina L. Madda & Nicky Glasswell
2022. Redesigning Reading in and for the Disciplines. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy 66:2  pp. 122 ff. DOI logo
Grabe, William & Fredricka L. Stoller
2020. Teaching Reading: Foundations and Practices. In The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics,  pp. 1 ff. DOI logo
Gruhn, Sophie, Eliane Segers & Ludo Verhoeven
2020. Moderating role of reading comprehension in children's word learning with context versus pictures. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 36:1  pp. 29 ff. DOI logo
Hessel, Annina K., Kate Nation & Victoria A. Murphy
2021. Comprehension Monitoring during Reading: An Eye-tracking Study with Children Learning English as an Additional Language. Scientific Studies of Reading 25:2  pp. 159 ff. DOI logo
Hoffman, James V., Sonia Q. Cabell, Sandra Barrueco, Etta R. Hollins & P. David Pearson
2021. Critical Issues in the Science of Reading: Striving for a Wide-Angle View in Research. Literacy Research: Theory, Method, and Practice 70:1  pp. 87 ff. DOI logo
Hughes-Berheim, Sarah S., Spyridoula Cheimariou, John F. Shelley-Tremblay, Margaret M. Doheny & Laura M. Morett
2022. Extending Gesture’s Impact on Word Learning to Reading: A Self-Paced Reading Study. Discourse Processes 59:8  pp. 646 ff. DOI logo
Inoue, Tomohiro, George K. Georgiou & Rauno Parrila
2023. The growth trajectories of morphological awareness and its predictors. Applied Psycholinguistics 44:5  pp. 699 ff. DOI logo
Li, Miao, George Georgiou, John R. Kirby, Jan C. Frijters, Wei Zhao & Tingzhao Wang
2023. Reading Fluency in Chinese Children With Reading Disabilities and/or ADHD: A Key Role for Morphology. Journal of Learning Disabilities 56:6  pp. 467 ff. DOI logo
Metsala, Jamie L.
2023. Longitudinal contributions of morphological awareness, listening comprehension, and gains in word reading fluency to later word- and text-reading fluency. Frontiers in Education 8 DOI logo
Metsala, Jamie L., Rauno Parrila, Nicole J. Conrad & S. Hélène Deacon
2019. Morphological awareness and reading achievement in university students. Applied Psycholinguistics 40:03  pp. 743 ff. DOI logo
Metsala, Jamie L., Erin Sparks, Margaret David, Nicole Conrad & S. Hélène Deacon
2021. What is the best way to characterise the contributions of oral language to reading comprehension: listening comprehension or individual oral language skills?. Journal of Research in Reading 44:3  pp. 675 ff. DOI logo
Oakhill, Jane
2020. Four Decades of Research into Children’s Reading Comprehension: A Personal Review. Discourse Processes 57:5-6  pp. 402 ff. DOI logo
Psyridou, Maria, Asko Tolvanen, Pekka Niemi, Marja-Kristiina Lerkkanen, Anna-Maija Poikkeus & Minna Torppa
2023. Development of silent reading fluency and reading comprehension across grades 1 to 9: unidirectional or bidirectional effects between the two skills?. Reading and Writing 36:8  pp. 1969 ff. DOI logo
Rigobon, Valeria M., Nuria Gutiérrez, Ashley A. Edwards, Nancy Marencin, Matt Cooper Borkenhagen, Laura M. Steacy & Donald L. Compton
2024. Modeling Item-Level Spelling Variance in Adults: Providing Further Insights into Lexical Quality. Scientific Studies of Reading 28:2  pp. 120 ff. DOI logo
Whyatt, Bogusława, Olga Witczak, Ewa Tomczak-Łukaszewska & Olha Lehka-Paul
2023. The proof of the translation process is in the reading of the target text: An eyetracking reception study. Ampersand 11  pp. 100149 ff. DOI logo
Young, Chase & Evan Ortlieb
2018. Implementing Readers Theater in Secondary Classrooms. Reading Psychology 39:8  pp. 879 ff. DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 12 april 2024. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.