Chapter published in:
Theories of Reading Development
Edited by Kate Cain, Donald L. Compton and Rauno K. Parrila
[Studies in Written Language and Literacy 15] 2017
► pp. 147168
References

References

Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D., & Quesada, J. F.
(2006) Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17, 814–823. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Amenta, S., & Crepaldi, D.
(2012) Morphological processing as we know it: an analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Apfelbaum, K. S., Hazeltine, E., & McMurray, B.
(2013) Statistical learning in reading: Variability in irrelevant letters helps children learn phonics skills. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1348–1365. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C.
(2012) Statistical learning is lasting and consistent over time. Neuroscience Letters, 517, 133–135. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K.
(1992) The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 335–345. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blythe, H. I.
(2014) Developmental changes in eye movements and visual information encoding associated with learning to read. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 201–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Blythe, H. I., Liang, F., Zang, C., Wang, J., Yan, G., Bai, X., & Liversedge, S. P.
(2012) Inserting spaces into Chinese text helps readers to learn new words: An eye movement study. Journal of Memory and Language, 67, 241–254. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosse, M., Chaves, N., Largy, P., & Valdois, S.
(2015) Orthographic learning during reading: the role of whole-word visual processing. Journal of Research in Reading, 38, 141–158. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bosse, M.-L., & Valdois, S.
(2009) Influence of the visual attention span on child reading performance: a cross-sectional study. Journal of Research in Reading, 32, 230–253. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A.
(2005) Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words. Cognition, 97, B45–B54. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Miller, R.
(2007) Correlates of orthographic learning in third-grade children’s silent reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 115–128. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Muller, D.
(2005) Phonological recoding and rapid orthographic learning in third-graders’ silent reading: A critical test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 203–219. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brusnighan, S. M., Morris, R. K., Folk, J. R., & Lowell, R.
(2014) The role of phonology in incidental vocabulary acquisition during silent reading. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 871–892. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P., & Nunes, T.
(2004) Morphology and spelling. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children’s literacy (pp. 91–118). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burt, J. S.
(2006) What is orthographic processing skill and how does it relate to word identification in reading? Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 400–417. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Hulslander, J., Wadsworth, S., DeFries, J. C., … Samuelsson, S.
(2008) A behaviour-genetic analysis of orthographic learning, spelling and decoding. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 8–21. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K.
(2007) Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 165–182. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Nation, K.
(2006) How does orthographic learning happen? In S. Andrews (Ed.), From inkmarks to ideas: Current issues in lexical processing (pp.151–179). Hove, England: Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T. & Rohrer, D.
(2006) Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 354–380. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J.
(2001) DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, A. E.
(2006) Accounting for children’s orthographic learning while reading text: Do children self-teach? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 56–77. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L.
(2002) Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E.
(1990) Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 733–740. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Davis, C. J., & Lupker, S. J.
(2006) Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 668–687.Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Castles, A.
(2012) Chicken or egg? Untangling the relationship between orthographic processing skill and reading accuracy. Cognition, 122, 110–117. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, P. F., & Share, D. L.
(2007) Orthographic learning during oral and silent reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 55–71. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, P. F., Bitter, D. J. L., van Setten, M., & Marinus, E.
(2009) Does phonological recoding occur during silent reading, and is it necessary for orthographic learning? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 267–282. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Duff, F. J., & Hulme, C.
(2012) The Role of Children’s Phonological and Semantic Knowledge in Learning to Read Words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 504–525. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ehri, L. C.
(1992) Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to decoding. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman, (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
(1995) Phases of development of learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2005) Learning to read words: Theories, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U.
(1985) Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–330). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gaskell, M. G., & Dumay, N.
(2003) Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words. Cognition, 89, 105–132. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S.
(1999) Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491–528. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, L. M., Weighall, A. R., Brown, H., & Gareth Gaskell, M.
(2012) Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep in children. Developmental Science, 15, 674–687. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Goetz, K., Gooch, D., Adams, J., & Snowling, M. J.
(2007) Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 150–166. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F. & Share D. L.
(1983) Phonological recoding and reading acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 4, 103–147 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, H. S. S. L., Wonnacott, E., Forbes, P., & Nation, K.
(2014) Becoming a written word: Eye movements reveal order of acquisition effects following incidental exposure to new words during silent reading. Cognition, 133, 238–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kohnen, S., & Castles, A.
(2013) Pirates at parties: Letter position processing in developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 91–107. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Kyte, C. S., & Johnson, C. J.
(2006) The role of phonological recoding in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 166–185. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Landi, N., Perfetti, C. A., Bolger, D. J., Dunlap, S., & Foorman, B. R.
(2006) The role of discourse context in developing word form representations: A paradoxical relation between reading and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 114–133. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Litt, R. A., de Jong, P. F., van Bergen, E., & Nation, K.
(2013) Dissociating crossmodal and verbal demands in paired associate learning (PAL): What drives the PAL – reading relationship? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 137–149. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Litt, R. A., & Nation, K.
(2014) The nature and specificity of paired associate learning deficits in children with dyslexia. Journal of Memory and Language, 71, 71–88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lowell, R., & Morris, R. K.
(2013) Word length effects on novel words: Evidence from eye movements. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 179–189. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Martin-Chang, S. L., Levy, B. A., & O’Neil, S.
(2007) Word acquisition, retention, and transfer: Findings from contextual and isolated word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 37–56. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McClung, N. A., O’Donnell, C. R., & Cunningham, A. E.
(2012) Orthographic learning and the development of visual word recognition. In J. S. Adelman (Ed.) Visual word recognition, Volume 2 (pp.173–195). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
McKague, M., Pratt, C., & Johnston, M. B.
(2001) The effect of oral vocabulary on reading visually novel words: A comparison of the dual-route-cascaded and triangle frameworks. Cognition, 80, 231–262. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McKay, A., Davis, C., Savage, G., & Castles, A.
(2008) Semantic involvement in reading aloud: evidence from a nonword training study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1495–1517.Google Scholar
Messbauer, V. C. L., & de Jong, P. F.
(2006) Effects of visual and phonological distinctness on visual-verbal paired-associate learning in Dutch dyslexic and normal readers. Reading and Writing, 19, 393–426. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G.
(2011) To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 267–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., Angell, P., & Castles, A.
(2007) Orthographic learning via self-teaching in children learning to read English: Effects of exposure, durability, and context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 71–84. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Cocksey, J.
(2009a) The relationship between knowing a word and reading it aloud in children’s word reading development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 296–308. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Beginning readers activate semantics from sub-word orthography. Cognition, 110, 273–278. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, G., & Fraser, J. R.
(2009) What exactly is a yait anyway: The role of semantics in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 239–251. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., & Deacon, S. H.
(2008) The timing and mechanisms of children’s use of morphological information in spelling: a review of evidence from French and English. Cognitive Development, 23, 339–359. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A.
(2001) Implicit learning out of the lab: The case of orthographic regularities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 401–426. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, A.
(1986) Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C.
(2007) Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pritchard, S., Coltheart, M., Marinus, E., & Castles, A.
submitted). A computational model of the self-teaching hypothesis based on the dual-route cascaded model of reading.
(2014) A new computational model of learning to read via self-teaching. 55th Psychonomic Society Annual Meeting, Long Beach, CA, November 20-23, 2014: Volume 19 [Abstracts] – Part 1, 143. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Qiao, X., & Forster, K. I.
(2013) Novel word lexicalization and the prime lexicality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1064–1074.Google Scholar
Ricketts, J., Davies, R., Masterson, J., Stuart, M. & Duff, F. J.
submitted). Probing the nature of the relationship between semantic knowledge and word reading: Evidence from six-year-old children.
Romberg, A. R., & Saffran, J. R.
(2010) Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 906–914.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Newport, E., Aslin, R. N., Tunick, R. & Barrueco, S.
(1997) Incidental language learning: Listening (and learning) out of the corner of your ear. Psychological Science, 8, 101–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L.
(1989) A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Share, D. L.
(1995) Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1999) Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Orthographic learning at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 267–298. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008a) On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2008b) Orthographic learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 31–82. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, M., Kaschak, M. P., Jones, J. L., & Lonigan, C. J.
(2015) Statistical learning is related to early literacy-related skills. Reading and Writing, 28, 467–490. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F.
(1989) Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 402–433. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stuart, M., Masterson, J., & Dixon, M.
(2000) Spongelike acquisition of sight vocabulary in beginning readers? Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 12–27. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tamura, N., Castles, A. & Nation, K.
in press). New words on the block: reading stories induces lexical competition effects in orthographic neighbours in developing readers. Cognition.
Taylor, J. S. H., Plunkett, K., & Nation, K.
(2011) The influence of consistency, frequency, and semantics on learning to read: An artificial orthography paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 60–76.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B.
(2006) Spelling as statistical learning: Using consonantal context to spell vowels. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 642–652. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, H.-C., Castles, A., & Nickels, L.
(2012) Word regularity affects orthographic learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 856–864. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, H.-C., Castles, A., Nickels, L., & Nation, K.
(2011) Context effects on orthographic learning of regular and irregular words. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 39–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wang, H.-C., Nickels, L., Nation, K., & Castles, A.
(2013) Predictors of orthographic learning of regular and irregular words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 369–384. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Warmington, M., & Hulme, C.
(2012) Phoneme awareness, visual-verbal paired-associate learning, and rapid automatized naming as predictors of individual differences in reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 45–62. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Windfuhr, K. L., & Snowling, M. J.
(2001) The Relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 160–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wochna, K. L., & Juhasz, B. J.
(2013) Context length and reading novel words: An eye-movement investigation. British Journal of Psychology, 104, 347–363. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Lété, B., & Grainger, J.
(2014) Orthographic and phonological contributions to reading development: Tracking developmental trajectories using masked priming. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1026–1036. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U.
(2005) Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A Psycholinguistic Grain Size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., Dufau, S., & Grainger, J.
(2010) Rapid processing of letters, digits and symbols: what purely visual-attentional deficit in developmental dyslexia? Developmental Science, 13, F8–F14. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Zorzi, M.
(2014) Modelling reading development through phonological decoding and self-teaching: implications for dyslexia. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 369.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 18 other publications

Bønnerup, Katrine H., Anne Leth Pedersen, Ethan Weed & Rauno Parrila
2019. Differences in the literacy skills of Danish dyslexic students in two types of higher education programmes. Dyslexia 25:2  pp. 173 ff. Crossref logo
Castles, Anne, Kathleen Rastle & Kate Nation
2018. Ending the Reading Wars: Reading Acquisition From Novice to Expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 19:1  pp. 5 ff. Crossref logo
Compton, Donald L.
2021. Focusing Our View of Dyslexia Through a Multifactorial Lens: A Commentary. Learning Disability Quarterly 44:3  pp. 225 ff. Crossref logo
Compton, Donald L., Jennifer K. Gilbert, Devin M. Kearns & Richard K. Olson
2020. Using an item-specific predictor to test the dimensionality of the orthographic choice task. Annals of Dyslexia 70:2  pp. 243 ff. Crossref logo
Davis, Dennis S., Jill S. Jones & Courtney Samuelson
2021. Is It Time for a Hard Conversation about Cueing Systems and Word Reading in Teacher Education?. Reading & Writing Quarterly 37:4  pp. 301 ff. Crossref logo
Elleman, Amy M., Laura M. Steacy & Donald L. Compton
2019. The Role of Statistical Learning in Word Reading and Spelling Development: More Questions Than Answers. Scientific Studies of Reading 23:1  pp. 1 ff. Crossref logo
Espinas, Daniel R., Min Wang & Yixun Li
2020.  In Handbook of Research on Cultivating Literacy in Diverse and Multilingual Classrooms [Advances in Educational Technologies and Instructional Design, ],  pp. 82 ff. Crossref logo
Ginestet, Emilie, Jalyssa Shadbolt, Rebecca Tucker, Marie‐Line Bosse & S. Hélène Deacon
2021. Orthographic learning and transfer of complex words: insights from eye tracking during reading and learning tasks. Journal of Research in Reading 44:1  pp. 51 ff. Crossref logo
Ginestet, Emilie, Sylviane Valdois, Julien Diard & Marie-Line Bosse
2020. Orthographic learning of novel words in adults: effects of exposure and visual attention on eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 32:8  pp. 785 ff. Crossref logo
Hoover, Wesley A. & William E. Tunmer
2020.  In The Cognitive Foundations of Reading and Its Acquisition [Literacy Studies, 20],  pp. 41 ff. Crossref logo
Li, Luan, Eva Marinus, Anne Castles & Hua-Chen Wang
2021. Oral vocabulary affects children’s orthographic learning in Chinese. Reading and Writing 34:6  pp. 1369 ff. Crossref logo
Li, Luan, Eva Marinus, Anne Castles, Miao-Ling Hsieh & Hua-Chen Wang
2021. Semantic and Phonological Decoding in Children’s Orthographic Learning in Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading 25:4  pp. 319 ff. Crossref logo
Petscher, Yaacov, Sonia Q. Cabell, Hugh W. Catts, Donald L. Compton, Barbara R. Foorman, Sara A. Hart, Christopher J. Lonigan, Beth M. Phillips, Christopher Schatschneider, Laura M. Steacy, Nicole Patton Terry & Richard K. Wagner
2020. How the Science of Reading Informs 21st‐Century Education. Reading Research Quarterly 55:S1 Crossref logo
Steacy, Laura M.
2020. Capitalizing on the promise of item-level analyses to inform new understandings of word reading development. Annals of Dyslexia 70:2  pp. 153 ff. Crossref logo
Steacy, Laura M., Ashley A. Edwards, Jay G. Rueckl, Yaacov Petscher & Donald L. Compton
2021. Modeling and Visualizing the Codevelopment of Word and Nonword Reading in Children From First Through Fourth Grade: Informing Developmental Trajectories of Children With Dyslexia. Child Development 92:3 Crossref logo
Tamura, Niina, Anne Castles & Kate Nation
2017. Orthographic learning, fast and slow: Lexical competition effects reveal the time course of word learning in developing readers. Cognition 163  pp. 93 ff. Crossref logo
Wass, Malin, Teresa Y. C. Ching, Linda Cupples, Hua-Chen Wang, Björn Lyxell, Louise Martin, Laura Button, Miriam Gunnourie, Isabelle Boisvert, Catherine McMahon & Anne Castles
2019. Orthographic Learning in Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 50:1  pp. 99 ff. Crossref logo
Wilcox, Gabrielle, Alena Galilee, Julia Stamp, Erica Makarenko & Frank P. MacMaster
2020. The Importance of Research on Integrating Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) with Evidence-Based Reading Interventions. Journal of Pediatric Neuropsychology 6:4  pp. 218 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 13 september 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.