Article published in:
Pragmatic Development in First Language Acquisition
Edited by Danielle Matthews
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 10] 2014
► pp. 199218
References
Ambridge, B. & Lieven, E.V.M.
(2011)  Child Language Acquisition:Contrasting Theoretical Approaches . Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, J.E.
(2008) THE BACON not the bacon: How children and adults understand accented and unaccented noun phrases, Cognition , 108, 69–99. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Afshordi, N., Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
(2011) Responses to felicitous/infelicitous uses of contrastive stress in online processing of adjective-noun phrase by 36-month-olds. Poster presented at 2011 Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting . Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Balog, H.L., & Snow, D.
(2007) The adaptation and application of relational and independent analyses for intonation production in young children. Journal of Phonetics , 35, 118–133. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beckman, M.E.
(1996) The parsing of prosody. Language and Cognitive Processes , 11, 17–67. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Beebe, B., Alson, D., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S., & Crown, C.
(1988) Vocal congruence in mother-infant play. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research , 17(3), 245–259. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E.
(1999) Cognition and language: Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development , 70, 636–644. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bibyk, S., Ito, K., Wagner, L., & Speer, S.
(2009) Children can use contrastive pitch accent in on-line processing. Paper presented at Boston University Conference on Language Development 34 , Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Bloom, K.
(1988) Quality of adult vocalizations affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language , 15, 469–480. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K., Russell, A., & Wassenberg, K.
(1987) Turntaking affects the quality of infants vocalizations. Journal of Child Language , 14, 211–227. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R.
(1973)  A First Language: The Early Stages . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Castellanos, I.
(2011)  Infants’ Selective Attention to Faces and Prosody of Speech: The Roles of Intersensory Redundancy and Exploratory Time . Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Florida International University.Google Scholar
Chen, A.
(2011a) The developmental path to phonological focus-marking in Dutch. In S. Frota, E. Gorka, & P. Prieto (Eds.), Prosodic Categories: Production, Perception and Comprehension (pp. 93–109). Dordrecht: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011b) Tuning information packaging: Intonational realization of topic and focus in child Dutch. Journal of Child Language , 38, 1055–1083. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Chen, A., & Fikkert, P.
(2007) Intonation of early two-word utterances in Dutch. In J. Trouvain, & W.J. Barry (Eds.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences , 315–320.Google Scholar
Christophe, A., Mehler, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
(2001) Perception of prosodic boundary correlates by newborn infants. Infancy , 23, 385–394. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Conboy, B.T., Sommerville, J.A., & Kuhl, P.K.
(2008) Cognitive control factors in speech perception at 11 months. Developmental Psychology , 44(5), 1505–1512. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Swinney, D.A.
(1987) Prosody and the development of comprehension. Journal of Child Language , 14, 145–167. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cruttenden, A.
(1985) Intonation comprehension in ten-year-olds. Journal of Child Language , 12, 643–661. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dahan, D., Tanenhaus, M.K., & Chambers, C.G.
(2002) Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language , 47, 292–314. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
DePaolis, R.A., Vihman, M.M. & Kunnari, S.
(2008) Prosody in production at the onset of word use: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Phonetics , 36, 406–422. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
D’Imperio, M.
(1997) Narrow focus and focal accent in the Neapolitan variety of Italian. Proceedings for Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications , 87–90.
Dore, J.
1975Holophrases, speech acts and language universals. Journal of Child Language , 2, 20–40. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Erickson, D.
(1998) Effects of contrastive emphasis on jaw opening. Phonetica , 55, 147–169. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A.
(1993) Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development , 64, 657–674. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A., & Mazzie, C.
(1991) Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults. Developmental Psychology , 27(2), 209–221. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A., Friedrich, M., & Christophe, A.
(2007) Brain responses in 4-month-old infants are already language specific. Current Biology , 17, 1208–1211. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Frota, S.
(2000)  Prosody and Focus in European Portuguese: Phonological Phrasing and Intonation . New York, NY: Garland.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B.
(1990) Pointing, naming, and talk about objects: Referential behavior in children and mothers. First Language , 10, 231–242. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R.M., Mervis, C.B. & Hirsh-Pasek, K.
(1994) Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language , 21(1), 125–155. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Grassmann, S., & Tomasello, M.
(2007) Two-year-olds use primary sentence accent to learn new words. Journal of child Language , 34, 677–687. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2010) Prosodic stress on a word directs 24-month-olds’ attention to a contextually new referent. Journal of Pragmatics , 42, 3098–3105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Harris, J.
(1990)  Early Language Development: Implications for Clinical and Educational Practice . New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Henderson. J.M., & Ferreira, F.
(2004) Scene perception for psycholinguists. In J.M. Henderson & F. Ferreira (Eds.), The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual World (pp. 1–58). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Herbst, L.E.
(2007) German 5-year-olds’ intonational marking of information status. Paper presented at 16th International Congress of Phonetic Science , Saarbrücken, Germany.Google Scholar
Höhle, B., Berger, F., Müller, A., Schmitz, M., & Weissenborn, J.
(2009) Focus particles in children’s language: Production and comprehension of Auch ‘Also’ in German learners from 1 year to 4 years of age. Language Acquisition , 16, 36–66. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Crossref
Hornby, P., & Hass, W.
(1970) Use of contrastive stress by preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research , 13, 395–399.Google Scholar
Ito, K.
(2002)  The Interaction of Focus and Lexical Pitch Accent in Speech Production and Dialogue Comprehension: Evidence from Japanese and Basque . Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.Google Scholar
Ito, K., Bibyk, S., Wagner, L., & Speer, S.R.
in press). Interpretation of contrastive pitch accent in 6- to 11-year-old English speaking children (and adults). Journal of Child Language . http://​dx​.doi​.org/ DOI: Crossref
Ito, K., Jincho, N., Minai, U., Yamane, N., & Mazuka, R.
(2008) Use of emphatic intonation for contrast resolution in Japanese: Adults vs. 6-yr olds. Poster presented to the 21th Annual CUNY Conference, Chapel Hill, NC.Google Scholar
(2012) Intonation facilitates contrast resolution: Evidence from Japanese adults & 6-year olds. Journal of Memory and Language , 66(1), 265–284. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ito, K., Jincho, N., Mazuka, R., Yamane, N. & Minai, U.
(2007) Effect of contrastive intonation in discourse comprehension in Japanese: An eye tracking study with adults and 6-yr olds. Poster presented to the 20th Annual CUNY Conference, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Ito, K. & Speer, S.R.
(2008) Anticipatory effect of intonation: Eye movements during instructed visual search. Journal of Memory & Language , 58, 541–573. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2011) Semantically-independent but contextually-dependent interpretation of contrastive accent. In Prieto, P., Frota, S., & Elordieta, G. (Eds.) Prosodic Categories: Production, Perception and Comprehension (pp. 69–92). Berlin: Springer. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A.D., & Hewes, A.K.
(1999) Genetically dissociated components of working memory: Evidence from Down’s and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia , 37, 637–651. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jasnow, M., & Feldstein, S.
(1986) Adult-like temporal characteristics of mother-infant interactions. Child Development , 57, 754–761. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jun, S.-A.
(Ed.) (2005)  Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing . Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jun, S-A.
(2012) Prosodic typology based on intonational phonology. Talk presented at Workshop on Prosodic Annotation . Ohio State University, April 2012.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., & Aslin, R.N.
(1995) Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology , 29, 1–23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Cutler, A., & Redanz, N.J.
(1993) Infants’ preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development , 64, 675–687. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P.W., Houston, D.M., & Newsome, M.
(1999) The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology , 39, 159–207. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A.
(1979)  A Functional Approach to Child Language: A Study of Determiners and Reference . Cambridge: CUP.Google Scholar
(1981) The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In W. Deutch (Ed.), The Child’s Construction of Language (pp. 121–147). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kitamura, C. & Lam, C.
(2009) Age-specific preferences for infant-directed affective intent. Infancy , 14, 1–24. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, R.D.
(2008)  Intonational Phonology (2nd Ed.). Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lalonde, C.E., & Werker, J.F.
(1995) Cognitive influences on crosslanguage speech perception in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development , 18, 459–475. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Lehiste, I.
(1970)  Suprasegmentals . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, C.N., & Thompson, S.A.
(1977) The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language , 4, 185–199. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, P.
(1967)  Intonation, Perception and Language . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Macwhinney, B., & Bates, E.
(1978) Sentential devices for conveying givenness and newness: A cross-cultural development study. Journal of Verbal Learning Verbal Behaviour , 17, 539–558. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mandel, D.R., Jusczyk, P.W. & Kemler Nelson, D.G.
(1994) Does sentential prosody help infants organize and remember speech information? Cognition , 53, 155–180. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Matychuk, P.
(2005) The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: A case study. Language Sciences , 27, 301–379. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
McCann, J., & Peppé, S.
(2003) Prosody in autistic spectrum disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders , 38, 325–350. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J. & Christophe, A.
(1995) Maturation and learning of language in the first year of life. In M.S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences (pp. 943–954). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P.W., Lambertz, G., Halsted, N., Bertoncini, J., & Amiel-Tison, C.
(1988) A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition , 29, 143–178. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C.B., & John, A.E.
(2010) Cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with Williams Syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C (Seminars in Medical Genetics), 154C , 229–248. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Minai, U., Jincho, N., Yamane, N., & Mazuka, R.
(2012) What hinders child semantic computation: Children’s universal quantification and the development of cognitive control. Journal of Child Language , 39(5), 919–956. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Müller, A., Höhle, B., Schmitz, M., & Weissenborn, J.
(2009) Information structural constraints on children’s early language production: The acquisition of focus particle auch (‘also’) in German-learning 12- to 36-month-olds. First Language , 29(4), 373–399. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Floccia, C., & Bertoncini, J.
(1998) Discrimination of pitch contours by neonates. Infant Behavior and Development , 21, 779–784. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peppé, S.J.E.
(2009a) Why is prosody in speech-language pathology so difficult? International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology , 11(4), 258–271. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2009b) Aspects of identifying prosodic impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology , 11(4), 332–338. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M.
(2007) Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research , 50, 1015–1028. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J.
(1990) The meaning of intonational contours in interpretation of discourse. In P. Cohen, J., Morgan, J., & M. Pollack (Eds.), Intentions in Communication . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M.M.
(2012) Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 221–257. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, V.J., Ingram, S.M., Lamar, M., & Green, R.C.
(1996) Prosody impairment and associated affective and behavioral disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology , 47, 1482–1488. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Russell, J.
(2004)  What is Language Development?: Rationalist, Empiricist, and Pragmatist Approaches to the Acquisition of Syntax . Oxford: OUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sakkalou, E., & Gattis, M.
(2012) Infants infer intentions from prosody. Cognitive Development , 27, 1–16. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Sekerina, I.E., & Trueswell, J.C.
(2012) Interactive processing of contrastive expressions by Russian children. First Language , 32(1–2), 63–87. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snedeker, J., & Yuan, S.
(2008) Effects of prosodic and lexical constraints on parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language , 58, 574–608. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C.E.
(1995) Issues in the study of input: Finetuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The Handbook of Child Language: The Spoken Language, Early Speech Development (pp. 180–193). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Snow, D.
(1998) Children’s imitation of intonation contours: Are rising tones more difficult than falling tones? Journal of Speech-Language and Hearing Research , 41, 576–587.Google Scholar
(2002) Intonation in the monosyllabic utterances of 1-year-olds. Infant Behavior and Development , 24, 393–407. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2004) Falling intonation in the one- and two-syllable vocalizations of one-year-olds. Journal of Phonetics , 32, 373–393. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2006) Regression and reorganization of intonation between 6 and 23 months. Child Development , 77(2), 281–296. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Soderstrom, M., Kemler Nelson, D.G., & Jusczyk, P.W.
(2005) Six-month-olds recognize clauses embedded in different passages of fluent speech. Infant Behavior and Development , 28, 87–94. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Solan, L.
(1980) Contrastive stress and children’s interpretation of pronouns. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research , 23, 688–698.Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V., & Setter, J.
(2009) Conditions in which prosodic impairments occur. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology , 11(4), 293–297. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Setter, J., & van Ewijk, L.
(2007) Intonation abilities of children with Williams Syndrome: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research , 50, 1606–1617. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Thiessen, E.D., & Saffran, J.R.
(2003) When cues collide: Use of stress and statistical cues to word boundaries by 7- to 9-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology , 39, 706–716. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M.
(2001) The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences , 10(1), 69–88. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(2003)  Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J.
(1983) Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language 4 , 197–212. Crossref
Tosto, G., Gasparini, M., Lenzi, G.L., & Bruno, G.
(2011) Prosodic impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: Assessment and clinical relevance. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences , 23, E21–E23. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, J.C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N.M., & Logrip, M.L.
(1999) The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition , 73, 89–134. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Vaish, A. & Striano, T.
(2004) Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science , 7, 261–269. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., Arnold, J.E., & Tanenhaus, M.K.
(2005) Not just given and new: The effects of discourse and task based constraints on acoustic prominence. Poster presented at the 18th CUNY human sentence processing conference , Tucson, AZ.Google Scholar
Weber, A., Braun, B., & Crocker, M.W.
(2006) Finding referents in time: Eye-tracking evidence for the role of contrastive accents. Language and Speech , 49(3), 367–392. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wells, B., Peppé, S., & Goulandris, N.
(2004) Intonation development from five to thirteen. Journal of Child Language , 31, 749–778. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wieman, L.A.
(1976) Stress patterns in early child language. Journal of Child Language , 3, 283–286. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wonnacott, E., & Watson, D.G.
(2008) Acoustic emphasis in four year olds. Cognition , 107, 1093–1101. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Yi
(1999) Effects of tone and focus on the formation and alignment of f0 contours. Journal of Phonetics , 27, 55–105. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, P., Crain, S., & Zhan, L.
(2012) Sometimes children are as good as adults: The pragmatic use of prosody in children’s on-line processing. Journal of Memory and Language , 67(1), 149–164. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 1 other publications

Chen, Aoju
2015.  In The Acquisition of Reference [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 15],  pp. 83 ff. Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 26 february 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.