Part of
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 101114
References
Balog, H.L., & Roberts, F.D.
(2004) Perception of utterance relatedness during the first-word period. Journal of Child Language , 31, 837–854. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K.
(1975) Social elicitation of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 20, 51–58. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1984) Distinguishing between social reinforcement and social elicitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 19, 209–222. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
(1988) Quality of adult vocalizations affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language , 15, 469–480. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., & Williams, M.
(1995) The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language , 34, 383–398. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Burgoon, J.K., Buller, D.B., & Guerrero, L.K.
(1995) Interpersonal deception: IX. Effects of social skill and nonverbal communication on deception success and detection accuracy. Journal of Language and Social Psychology , 14, 289–311. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., Bobb, S.C., & Clark, E.V.
Under review). Turn taking, timing, and planning in early language acquisition.
Casillas, M., & Frank, M.C.
(2013) The development of predictive processes in children’s discourse understanding. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 299–304). Berlin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Clark, E.V.
(2007) How young children describe events in time. In G.B. Flores d’Arcais & W.J.M. Levelt (Eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (pp. 275–293). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
(2009)  First language acquisition , 2nd Ed . Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J.
(2008) Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language , 35, 349–372. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & FoxTree, J.E.
(2002) Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition , 84, 73–111. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K. Culbertson, J., & Alter, J.
(2006) Word-minimality, epenthesis, and coda licensing in the acquisition of English. Language and Speech , 49, 137–173. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., & Shatz, M.
(1989) Becoming a conversationalist despite or because of having an older sibling. Child Development , 60, 399–410. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S.
(1979) Children’s verbal turn taking. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental Pragmatics (pp. 391–414). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fitneva, S.A.
(2012) Questions and children’s social information gathering. In J.P. de Ruiter (Ed.), Questions: Formal, functional, and interactional perspectives (pp. 165–178). Cambridge: CUP. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Garvey, C., & Berninger, G.
(1981) Timing and turn taking in children’s conversations. Discourse Processes , 4, 27–57. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Gold, E.M.
(1967) Language Identification in the Limit. Information and Control , 10, 447–474. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Hudson Kam, C.L., & Edwards, N.A.
(2008) The use of uh and um by 3- and 4-year-old native English-speaking children: Not quite right, but not completely wrong. First Language , 28, 313­–327. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C.
(2006) On the human “interaction engine. In N.J. Enfield & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Lieven, E.V.M.
(1978) Conversations between mothers and young children: Individual differences and their possible implications for the study of language learning. In N. Waterson & C. Snow (Eds.), The development of communication: Social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition (pp. 173–187). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2012)  The CHILDES project . Retrieved from: [URL].Google Scholar
Mannle, S., & Tomasello, M.
(1987) Fathers, siblings, and the Bridge Hypothesis. Children’s Language , 6, 23–42.Google Scholar
Masataka, N.
(1993) Effects of contingent and noncontingent maternal stimulation on the vocal behaviour of three- to four-month-old Japanese infants. Journal of Child Language , 20, 303–312. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roberts, F., Francis, A.L., & Morgan, M.
(2006) The interaction of inter-turn silence with prosodic cues in listener perceptions of “trouble” in conversation. Speech Communication , 48, 1079–1093. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Roy, B., Frank, M.C., & Roy, D.
(2009) Exploring word learning in a high-density longitudinal corpus. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2106–2111). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A., & Jefferson, G.
(1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. Language , 50, 696–735. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Saylor, M.M., Baird, J.A., & Gallerani, C.
(2006) Telling others what’s new: Preschoolers’ adherence to the given-new contract. Journal of Cognition & Development , 7, 341–379. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Smith, V.L. & Clark, H.H.
(1993) On the course of answering questions. Journal of Memory and Language , 32, 25–38. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Snow, C.E.
(1977) The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language , 4, 1–22. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N.J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T. Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J.P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S.C.
(2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science , 106, 10587–10592.
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M.J.
(1986) Joint attention and early language. Child Development , 6, 1454–1463. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Uther, M., Knoll, M.A., & Burnham, D.
(2007) Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication , 49, 2–7. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Van Der Wege, M.M., & Ragatz, E.C.
(2004) Learning to be fluently disfluent. In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 1647). Chicago, IL: Cognitive Science Society.
Von Hofsten, C., Uhlig, H., Adell, M., & Kochukhova, O.
(2009) How children with autism look at events. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 3, 556–569. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H.M., & Lempers, J.D.
(1977) The naturalistic communicative abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development , 48, 1052–1057. DOI logoGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P.W.
(1983)  Formal principles of language acquisition . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Clark, Eve V.
2017. Chapter 2. Becoming social and interactive with language. In Social Environment and Cognition in Language Development [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 21],  pp. 19 ff. DOI logo
Levinson, Stephen C. & Francisco Torreira
2015. Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology 6 DOI logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 9 november 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.