Taking the floor on time
Delay and deferral in children’s turn taking
A key part of learning to speak with others is figuring out when to start talking and how to hold the floor in conversation. For young children, the challenge of planning a linguistic response can slow down their response latencies, making misunderstanding, repair, and loss of the floor more likely. Like adults, children can mitigate their delays by using fillers (e.g., uh and um) at the start of their turns. In this chapter I analyze the onset and development of fillers in five children’s spontaneous speech from ages 1;6–3;6. My findings suggest that children start using fillers by 2;0, and use them to effectively mitigate delay in making a response.
References
Balog, H.L., & Roberts, F.D.
(
2004)
Perception of utterance relatedness during the first-word period.
Journal of Child Language
, 31, 837–854.


Bloom, K.
(
1975)
Social elicitation of infant vocal behaviour.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
, 20, 51–58.


Bloom, K.
(
1984)
Distinguishing between social reinforcement and social elicitation.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
, 19, 209–222.


Bloom, K.
(
1988)
Quality of adult vocalizations affects the quality of infant vocalizations.
Journal of Child Language
, 15, 469–480.


Brennan, S.E., & Williams, M.
(
1995)
The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers.
Journal of Memory and Language
, 34, 383–398.


Burgoon, J.K., Buller, D.B., & Guerrero, L.K.
(
1995)
Interpersonal deception: IX. Effects of social skill and nonverbal communication on deception success and detection accuracy.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology
, 14, 289–311.


Casillas, M., Bobb, S.C., & Clark, E.V.
Under review).
Turn taking, timing, and planning in early language acquisition.
Casillas, M., & Frank, M.C.
(
2013)
The development of predictive processes in children’s discourse understanding. In
M. Knauff,
M. Pauen,
N. Sebanz, &
I. Wachsmuth (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
(pp. 299–304). Berlin: Cognitive Science Society.

Clark, E.V.
(
2007)
How young children describe events in time. In
G.B. Flores d’Arcais &
W.J.M. Levelt (Eds.),
Advances in psycholinguistics
(pp. 275–293). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Clark, E.V.
(
2009)
First language acquisition
,
2nd Ed
. Cambridge: CUP.


Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J.
(
2008)
Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground.
Journal of Child Language
, 35, 349–372.


Clark, H.H., & FoxTree, J.E.
(
2002)
Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking.
Cognition
, 84, 73–111.


Demuth, K. Culbertson, J., & Alter, J.
(
2006)
Word-minimality, epenthesis, and coda licensing in the acquisition of English.
Language and Speech
, 49, 137–173.


Dunn, J., & Shatz, M.
(
1989)
Becoming a conversationalist despite or because of having an older sibling.
Child Development
, 60, 399–410.


Ervin-Tripp, S.
(
1979)
Children’s verbal turn taking. In
E. Ochs &
B. Schieffelin (Eds.),
Developmental Pragmatics
(pp. 391–414). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Fitneva, S.A.
(
2012)
Questions and children’s social information gathering. In
J.P. de Ruiter (Ed.),
Questions: Formal, functional, and interactional perspectives
(pp. 165–178). Cambridge: CUP.


Garvey, C., & Berninger, G.
(
1981)
Timing and turn taking in children’s conversations.
Discourse Processes
, 4, 27–57.


Gold, E.M.
(
1967)
Language Identification in the Limit.
Information and Control
, 10, 447–474.


Hudson Kam, C.L., & Edwards, N.A.
(
2008)
The use of uh and um by 3- and 4-year-old native English-speaking children: Not quite right, but not completely wrong.
First Language
, 28, 313–327.


Levinson, S.C.
(
2006)
On the human “interaction engine”. In
N.J. Enfield &
S.C. Levinson (Eds.),
Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction
(pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.

Lieven, E.V.M.
(
1978)
Conversations between mothers and young children: Individual differences and their possible implications for the study of language learning. In
N. Waterson &
C. Snow (Eds.),
The development of communication: Social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition
(pp. 173–187). New York, NY: Wiley.

MacWhinney, B.
(
2012)
The CHILDES project
. Retrieved from:
[URL].

Mannle, S., & Tomasello, M.
(
1987)
Fathers, siblings, and the Bridge Hypothesis.
Children’s Language
, 6, 23–42.

Masataka, N.
(
1993)
Effects of contingent and noncontingent maternal stimulation on the vocal behaviour of three- to four-month-old Japanese infants.
Journal of Child Language
, 20, 303–312.


Roberts, F., Francis, A.L., & Morgan, M.
(
2006)
The interaction of inter-turn silence with prosodic cues in listener perceptions of “trouble” in conversation.
Speech Communication
, 48, 1079–1093.


Roy, B., Frank, M.C., & Roy, D.
(
2009)
Exploring word learning in a high-density longitudinal corpus. In
N. Taatgen &
H. van Rijn (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
(pp. 2106–2111). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.

Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A., & Jefferson, G.
(
1974)
A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation.
Language
, 50, 696–735.


Saylor, M.M., Baird, J.A., & Gallerani, C.
(
2006)
Telling others what’s new: Preschoolers’ adherence to the given-new contract.
Journal of Cognition & Development
, 7, 341–379.


Smith, V.L. & Clark, H.H.
(
1993)
On the course of answering questions.
Journal of Memory and Language
, 32, 25–38.


Snow, C.E.
(
1977)
The development of conversation between mothers and babies.
Journal of Child Language
, 4, 1–22.


Stivers, T., Enfield, N.J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T. Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J.P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S.C.
(
2009)
Universals and cultural variation in turn taking in conversation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science
, 106, 10587–10592.
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M.J.
(
1986)
Joint attention and early language.
Child Development
, 6, 1454–1463.


Uther, M., Knoll, M.A., & Burnham, D.
(
2007)
Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech.
Speech Communication
, 49, 2–7.


Van Der Wege, M.M., & Ragatz, E.C.
(
2004)
Learning to be fluently disfluent. In
K. Forbus,
D. Gentner, &
T. Regier (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
(p. 1647). Chicago, IL: Cognitive Science Society.
Von Hofsten, C., Uhlig, H., Adell, M., & Kochukhova, O.
(
2009)
How children with autism look at events.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
, 3, 556–569.


Wellman, H.M., & Lempers, J.D.
(
1977)
The naturalistic communicative abilities of two-year-olds.
Child Development
, 48, 1052–1057.


Wexler, K., & Culicover, P.W.
(
1983)
Formal principles of language acquisition
. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Cited by
Cited by 2 other publications
Levinson, Stephen C. & Francisco Torreira
2015.
Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language.
Frontiers in Psychology 6

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 9 november 2023. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers.
Any errors therein should be reported to them.