Article published in:
Language in Interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark
Edited by Inbal Arnon, Marisa Casillas, Chigusa Kurumada and Bruno Estigarribia
[Trends in Language Acquisition Research 12] 2014
► pp. 101114
References

References

Balog, H.L., & Roberts, F.D.
(2004) Perception of utterance relatedness during the first-word period. Journal of Child Language , 31, 837–854. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K.
(1975) Social elicitation of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 20, 51–58. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1984) Distinguishing between social reinforcement and social elicitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 19, 209–222. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
(1988) Quality of adult vocalizations affects the quality of infant vocalizations. Journal of Child Language , 15, 469–480. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, S.E., & Williams, M.
(1995) The feeling of another’s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language , 34, 383–398. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Burgoon, J.K., Buller, D.B., & Guerrero, L.K.
(1995) Interpersonal deception: IX. Effects of social skill and nonverbal communication on deception success and detection accuracy. Journal of Language and Social Psychology , 14, 289–311. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Casillas, M., Bobb, S.C., & Clark, E.V.
Under review). Turn taking, timing, and planning in early language acquisition.
Casillas, M., & Frank, M.C.
(2013) The development of predictive processes in children’s discourse understanding. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 299–304). Berlin: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Clark, E.V.
(2007) How young children describe events in time. In G.B. Flores d’Arcais & W.J.M. Levelt (Eds.), Advances in psycholinguistics (pp. 275–293). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
(2009)  First language acquisition , 2nd Ed . Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E.V., & Bernicot, J.
(2008) Repetition as ratification: How parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language , 35, 349–372. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H.H., & FoxTree, J.E.
(2002) Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking. Cognition , 84, 73–111. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Demuth, K. Culbertson, J., & Alter, J.
(2006) Word-minimality, epenthesis, and coda licensing in the acquisition of English. Language and Speech , 49, 137–173. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., & Shatz, M.
(1989) Becoming a conversationalist despite or because of having an older sibling. Child Development , 60, 399–410. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S.
(1979) Children’s verbal turn taking. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental Pragmatics (pp. 391–414). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fitneva, S.A.
(2012) Questions and children’s social information gathering. In J.P. de Ruiter (Ed.), Questions: Formal, functional, and interactional perspectives (pp. 165–178). Cambridge: CUP. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Garvey, C., & Berninger, G.
(1981) Timing and turn taking in children’s conversations. Discourse Processes , 4, 27–57. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Gold, E.M.
(1967) Language Identification in the Limit. Information and Control , 10, 447–474. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Hudson Kam, C.L., & Edwards, N.A.
(2008) The use of uh and um by 3- and 4-year-old native English-speaking children: Not quite right, but not completely wrong. First Language , 28, 313­–327. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S.C.
(2006) On the human “interaction engine. In N.J. Enfield & S.C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 39–69). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Lieven, E.V.M.
(1978) Conversations between mothers and young children: Individual differences and their possible implications for the study of language learning. In N. Waterson & C. Snow (Eds.), The development of communication: Social and pragmatic factors in language acquisition (pp. 173–187). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B.
(2012)  The CHILDES project . Retrieved from: www​.childes​.psy​.cmu​.edu0.Google Scholar
Mannle, S., & Tomasello, M.
(1987) Fathers, siblings, and the Bridge Hypothesis. Children’s Language , 6, 23–42.Google Scholar
Masataka, N.
(1993) Effects of contingent and noncontingent maternal stimulation on the vocal behaviour of three- to four-month-old Japanese infants. Journal of Child Language , 20, 303–312. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, F., Francis, A.L., & Morgan, M.
(2006) The interaction of inter-turn silence with prosodic cues in listener perceptions of “trouble” in conversation. Speech Communication , 48, 1079–1093. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Roy, B., Frank, M.C., & Roy, D.
(2009) Exploring word learning in a high-density longitudinal corpus. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2106–2111). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E.A., & Jefferson, G.
(1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. Language , 50, 696–735. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Saylor, M.M., Baird, J.A., & Gallerani, C.
(2006) Telling others what’s new: Preschoolers’ adherence to the given-new contract. Journal of Cognition & Development , 7, 341–379. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Smith, V.L. & Clark, H.H.
(1993) On the course of answering questions. Journal of Memory and Language , 32, 25–38. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C.E.
(1977) The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language , 4, 1–22. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N.J., Brown, P., Englert, C., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T. Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J.P., Yoon, K.-E., & Levinson, S.C.
(2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science , 106, 10587–10592.
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M.J.
(1986) Joint attention and early language. Child Development , 6, 1454–1463. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Uther, M., Knoll, M.A., & Burnham, D.
(2007) Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication , 49, 2–7. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Van Der Wege, M.M., & Ragatz, E.C.
(2004) Learning to be fluently disfluent. In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 1647). Chicago, IL: Cognitive Science Society.
Von Hofsten, C., Uhlig, H., Adell, M., & Kochukhova, O.
(2009) How children with autism look at events. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 3, 556–569. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H.M., & Lempers, J.D.
(1977) The naturalistic communicative abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development , 48, 1052–1057. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P.W.
(1983)  Formal principles of language acquisition . Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cited by

Cited by 2 other publications

Clark, Eve V.
2017.  In Social Environment and Cognition in Language Development [Trends in Language Acquisition Research, 21],  pp. 19 ff. Crossref logo
Levinson, Stephen C. & Francisco Torreira
2015. Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology 6 Crossref logo

This list is based on CrossRef data as of 27 february 2021. Please note that it may not be complete. Sources presented here have been supplied by the respective publishers. Any errors therein should be reported to them.